2021
DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2020.100973
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Young children’s moral judgments depend on the social relationship between agents

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
10
1

Year Published

2022
2022
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
1
10
1
Order By: Relevance
“…These non-effects contrast with previous research showing that both adults and children exhibit more negative attitudes toward transgressors when those transgressors are similar to themselves in age (Rottman et al, 2020), as well as research showing that children expect a child to receive more help from their parent than from their child-aged peers (Mammen et al, 2021). In the absence of additional evidence, we can only speculate about why children's responses-unlike adults' responses-were unaffected by the transgressor's age, and we offer some speculations in the General Discussion, below.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 99%
“…These non-effects contrast with previous research showing that both adults and children exhibit more negative attitudes toward transgressors when those transgressors are similar to themselves in age (Rottman et al, 2020), as well as research showing that children expect a child to receive more help from their parent than from their child-aged peers (Mammen et al, 2021). In the absence of additional evidence, we can only speculate about why children's responses-unlike adults' responses-were unaffected by the transgressor's age, and we offer some speculations in the General Discussion, below.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 99%
“…Once they were done with their decisions, children were probably reluctant to talk when E asked further questions. This is also in line with the literature which suggests children do not often answer open‐ended why‐questions (Mammen et al, 2021). Spontaneous meta‐talk, as compared to elicited meta‐talk, was more informative in terms of what actually guided children's reasoning and decision‐making.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…A teeny bit, a little bit, or a lot?” if participants responded “yes.” This resulted in a 4-point obligation scale for each witness for each story type (1 = No , 2 = Yes, a tiny bit , 3 = Yes, a little bit , 4 = Yes, a lot ). We decided to use the language “have to” as previous research has documented that children distinguish between what individuals have to do, prefer to do, and usually do (Kalish & Lawson, 2008; Mammen et al, 2021). In this way, this item is closest to conceptually measuring children’s sense of obligation.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some recent research finds that 3- to 5-year-olds in the U.S.—like older children in India—consider helping obligatory regardless of social–relational context (e.g., parent and acquaintance; Dahl et al 2020). Alternatively, recent work by Mammen and colleagues(2021) finds that 4- and 6-year-olds in Germany consider parents more likely to help than friends and tend to use more normative language (“have to”) to justify help on behalf of parents compared to friends. Supporting the notion that younger children take social relationships into account, research by Paulus and colleagues (2020) finds that 4- to 6-year-olds evaluate someone who helps a friend more positively than someone who helps a disliked other.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%