2022
DOI: 10.1037/dev0001349
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Young children do not perceive distributional fairness as a moral norm.

Abstract: Young children robustly distinguish between moral norms and conventional norms (Smetana, 1984;. In existing research, norms about the fair distribution of resources are by definition considered part of the moral domain; they are not distinguished from other moral norms such as those involving physical harm. Yet an understanding of fairness in resource distribution (hereafter, "fairness") emerges late in development and is culturally variable, raising the possibility that fairness may not fall squarely in the m… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
4
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
2
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…GROUP-TRANSCENDENT FAIRNESS Based on the research reviewed, we hypothesize that it would be mostly likely to observe adults and older children (age 8 and above), but not younger children, to endorse grouptranscendent fairness over ingroup support. This is consistent with the findings that children by age 8 start to think fairness transgressions as more serious than conventional violations (Yucel et al, 2022) and they prefer equal allocations over allocations that favor themselves (e.g., Blake et al, 2015;Blake & McAuliffe, 2011;Fehr et al, 2008;Sheskin et al, 2014). At the same time, group-transcendent fairness might also be culturally dependent, such that only people in western societies with strong emphasis on egalitarian values (Haidt & Kesebir, 2010;Henrich, 2020) would endorse it over ingroup support.…”
Section: Beyond Our Tribe: Developing a Normative Sense Of Group-tran...supporting
confidence: 80%
“…GROUP-TRANSCENDENT FAIRNESS Based on the research reviewed, we hypothesize that it would be mostly likely to observe adults and older children (age 8 and above), but not younger children, to endorse grouptranscendent fairness over ingroup support. This is consistent with the findings that children by age 8 start to think fairness transgressions as more serious than conventional violations (Yucel et al, 2022) and they prefer equal allocations over allocations that favor themselves (e.g., Blake et al, 2015;Blake & McAuliffe, 2011;Fehr et al, 2008;Sheskin et al, 2014). At the same time, group-transcendent fairness might also be culturally dependent, such that only people in western societies with strong emphasis on egalitarian values (Haidt & Kesebir, 2010;Henrich, 2020) would endorse it over ingroup support.…”
Section: Beyond Our Tribe: Developing a Normative Sense Of Group-tran...supporting
confidence: 80%
“…Based on the research reviewed, we hypothesize that it would be mostly likely to observe adults and older children (age 8 and above), but not younger children, to endorse group-transcendent fairness over ingroup support. This is consistent with the findings that children by age 8 start to think fairness transgressions as more serious than conventional violations (Yucel et al, 2022) and they prefer equal allocations over allocations that favor themselves (e.g., Blake et al, 2015; Blake & McAuliffe, 2011; Fehr et al, 2008; Sheskin et al, 2014). At the same time, group-transcendent fairness might also be culturally dependent, such that only people in western societies with strong emphasis on egalitarian values (Haidt & Kesebir, 2010; Henrich, 2020) would endorse it over ingroup support.…”
supporting
confidence: 89%
“…While our findings suggest a bidirectional tendency to generalize from moral norm violations from help/harm to fairness and vice versa, a descriptive characterization of the effect sizes across studies indicates that infants generalize more strongly from hindering behavior to unfairness ( d = 1.067) than they do from unfair behavior to hindering ( d = 0.815). Coupled with the fact that infants show a sensitivity to help/harm prior to when they show a sensitivity to fairness/unfairness ( Sommerville, 2022 ), and the fact that individuals tend to see hindering as more egregious than unfairness ( Yucel et al, 2022 ), these findings raise the possibility that the degree to which infants will generalize from a given moral behavior may vary according to the severity of that behavior. Future work can directly test this idea.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We also sought to compare the relative strength of these generalizations based on the original sub-domain. On the one hand, one might predict that infants will generalize more strongly from hindering versus unfairness given that hindering is often see as more egregious than unfairness ( Yucel et al, 2022 ), and that infants appear to be sensitive to help/harm before fairness/unfairness in their evaluations ( Sommerville, 2022 ). On the other hand, we might expect the opposing pattern given that some work suggests that while infants have baseline expectations for fair distributions, they do not have baseline expectations for helpful behavior (but see Hamlin et al, 2007 ; Lucca et al, 2018 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%