2014
DOI: 10.1037/a0037837
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Young children create partner-specific referential pacts with peers.

Abstract: In 2 studies, we investigated how peers establish a referential pact to call something, for example, a cushion versus a pillow (both equally felicitous). In Study 1, pairs of 4- and 6-year-old German-speaking peers established a referential pact for an artifact, for example, a woman's shoe, in a referential communication task. Six-year-olds, but not 4-year-olds, continued to use these same expressions with the same partner (even when they were overinformative) but shifted to simpler expressions, for example, s… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

1
17
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
(70 reference statements)
1
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In this sense, children’s adaptations for interacting with peers during early and middle childhood are very likely deferred adaptations, whose adaptive significance lies in the ways in which they prepare children for adulthood. During early and middle childhood children learn to make decisions with others—for example, about where to go or what to play (Köymen et al 2014). They learn to treat others fairly—for example, by sharing resources in a mutually satisfactory manner (Hamann et al 2011).…”
Section: A Composite Accountmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In this sense, children’s adaptations for interacting with peers during early and middle childhood are very likely deferred adaptations, whose adaptive significance lies in the ways in which they prepare children for adulthood. During early and middle childhood children learn to make decisions with others—for example, about where to go or what to play (Köymen et al 2014). They learn to treat others fairly—for example, by sharing resources in a mutually satisfactory manner (Hamann et al 2011).…”
Section: A Composite Accountmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…H&G should not be seen as entailing a commitment to a distinctly egocentric component to production (e.g., Köymen, Schmerse, Lieven, & Tomasello, ). On our view, utterance plans, from the earliest moments of speech planning, will indeed generally be influenced by information readily accessible from memory, and sometimes this will consist primarily of an individual's private knowledge.…”
Section: Clarifications and Extensions Of The Memory‐based Viewmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…After conversational partners develop a referential pact, if a new partner who was not involved in entrainment is introduced, the entrained term is less likely to be maintained by directors (Brennan and Clark, 1996 ) or expected by matchers (Metzing and Brennan, 2003 ; Brown-Schmidt, 2009 ). Recent work with typically-developing children shows that children as young as 4 years old maintain referential precedents with their peers in a partner–specific manner (Köymen et al, 2014 ) and that 3- and 4-year-olds expect adult speakers to maintain referential precedents in a partner–specific manner (Matthews et al, 2010 ; Graham et al, 2014 ). The mechanisms underlying the comprehension of referential precedents is an area of active debate, at the heart of which is whether high-level common ground inferences or low-level memory mechanisms (episodic priming and encoding cues) best explain the effects (Brennan and Hanna, 2009 ; Brown-Schmidt, 2009 ; Shintel and Keysar, 2009 ; Kronmüller and Barr, 2015 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The mechanisms underlying the comprehension of referential precedents is an area of active debate, at the heart of which is whether high-level common ground inferences or low-level memory mechanisms (episodic priming and encoding cues) best explain the effects (Brennan and Hanna, 2009 ; Brown-Schmidt, 2009 ; Shintel and Keysar, 2009 ; Kronmüller and Barr, 2015 ). The task we use stands somewhat outside this debate as it is a production task, and prior work on production used a different paradigm with familiar objects with known names rather than tangram stimuli (Brennan and Clark, 1996 ; Köymen et al, 2014 ). We view our task, where participants as directors need to create agreed upon terms for complex novel stimuli through interaction with a matcher, as one that inherently requires collaboration.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%