2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.05.020
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Working memory gating mechanisms explain developmental change in rule-guided behavior

Abstract: Cognitive control requires choosing contextual information to update into working memory (input gating), maintaining it there (maintenance) stable against distraction, and then choosing which subset of maintained information to use in guiding action (output gating). Recent work has raised the possibility that the development of rule-guided behavior, in the transition from childhood to adolescence, is linked specifically to changes in the gating components of working memory (Amso, Haas, McShane, & Badre, 2014).… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

5
48
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 34 publications
(54 citation statements)
references
References 75 publications
(127 reference statements)
5
48
1
Order By: Relevance
“…However, we did not observe the expected Age by Block effect on RTs seen in the Response and Feature tasks above [ F (2, 198) = 1.63, p = 0.20, ηp2 = 0.02] and previously demonstrated in Unger et al. (). Unger et al.…”
Section: Resultscontrasting
confidence: 81%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…However, we did not observe the expected Age by Block effect on RTs seen in the Response and Feature tasks above [ F (2, 198) = 1.63, p = 0.20, ηp2 = 0.02] and previously demonstrated in Unger et al. (). Unger et al.…”
Section: Resultscontrasting
confidence: 81%
“…None of the SES data analyses in this report were in Unger et al. (). Of the N = 141 participants tested, N = 114 contributed data to the Response task, N = 115 contributed full data to the Feature task, and N = 123 contributed data to the Dimension task.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 89%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…More complex rule representations are inherently more abstract (in that they apply across a wider range of situations) and allow for more flexibility and intentional control, as manifested in specific EF skills that continue to improve into early adulthood. These accounts are supported by research on the development of rule use [e.g., Amso, Haas, McShane & Badre, 2014;Bunge & Zelazo, 2006;Crone, Donohue, Honomichl, Wendelken, & Bunge;Unger, Ackerman, Chatham, Amso, & Badre, 2016;Zelazo, Muller, Frye, & Marcovitch, 2003], which shows clear age-related increases in abstraction, hierarchical complexity, and intentional control, including a progressive shift from more reactive to more proactive control over behavior [e.g., Doebel, Barker, Chevalier, Michaelson, Fisher, & Munakata, 2017]. In addition, there is evidence that the formulation and use of more complex rules that control the application of simpler rules (e.g., if color game, then if red, then it goes here) involves the recruitment of increasingly anterior regions of lateral prefrontal cortex into an increasingly complex, hierarchically arranged network of prefrontal cortex regions, where higher levels in the hierarchy operate on the products of lower levels [e.g., Badre & D'Esposito, 2007;Botvinick, 2008;Christoff & Gabrieli, 2000;Goldberg & Bilder, 1987;Koechlin, Ody, & Kouneiher, 2003;Ranti, Chatham, & Badre, 2015].…”
mentioning
confidence: 89%
“…This evidence comes from studies that are based on work of Badre, D'Esposito, and colleagues (Badre, ; Badre & D'Esposito, ; Badre, Kayser, & D'Esposito, ; Kayser & D'Esposito, ). This work indicates that children have the ability to use hierarchical rule sets to guide response categorizations (Amso, Haas, McShane, & Badre, ; Unger, Ackerman, Chatham, Amso, & Badre, ; Werchan et al, ). One of these studies is particularly relevant in the current context because Werchan et al () investigated whether 8‐month‐old infants can learn hierarchical rule sets.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 97%