2000
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.26.2.336
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Working-memory capacity, proactive interference, and divided attention: Limits on long-term memory retrieval.

Abstract: Abstract:Two experiments examined how individual differences in working-memory capacity (WM) relate -to proactive interference (PI) susceptibility. We tested high and low WM-span participants in a PI-buildup task under singletask or dual-task ("load") conditions. In Experiment 1, a finger-tapping task was imposed during encoding anti retrieval of each list; in Experiment 2, tapping was required during encoding or retrieval. In both experiments, low spans showed greater PI than did high spans under no load, but… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

45
498
5
8

Year Published

2003
2003
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 487 publications
(557 citation statements)
references
References 82 publications
45
498
5
8
Order By: Relevance
“…Differences in contextual distinctiveness have been shown to contribute to age differences in serial recall at the beginning (McCormack, Brown, Vousden, & Henson, 2000) and at the end of the life span (Maylor, Vousden, & Brown, 1999). On a more global level, more distinctive contexts also serve to distinguish the current memory set from those of previous trials, reducing proactive interference -this assumption could explain why WM capacity is correlated with the susceptibility to proactive interference (Kane & Engle, 2000).…”
Section: Correlations With Processing Speed (C2)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Differences in contextual distinctiveness have been shown to contribute to age differences in serial recall at the beginning (McCormack, Brown, Vousden, & Henson, 2000) and at the end of the life span (Maylor, Vousden, & Brown, 1999). On a more global level, more distinctive contexts also serve to distinguish the current memory set from those of previous trials, reducing proactive interference -this assumption could explain why WM capacity is correlated with the susceptibility to proactive interference (Kane & Engle, 2000).…”
Section: Correlations With Processing Speed (C2)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, individuals lower in working memory capacity may not be able to effectively suppress nonrelevant information while focusing on and deliberately elaborating the content of a presented persuasive message. Individuals low in working memory capacity are more vulnerable to interference effects, which results in weaker performance in secondary tasks compared with individuals high in working memory capacity (e.g., Conway, Cowan, & Bunting, 2001; Kane & Engle, 2000; Rosen & Engle, 1998). However, an extensive literature research did not yield any studies in which working memory capacity was empirically related to argument strength in the context of persuasion, neither for non-narrative nor for narrative texts.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Good working memory helps individuals keep competing considerations "online" (Kane & Engle, 2002), even whenfaced with other demands on cognitive resources (e.g., carrying on a conversation, considering peer's opinions). Without good working memory, multiple considerations are not as likely to be kept active or "online" for any decisions, and explicit memory retrieval is less effective (De Neys, Schaeken, & d'Ydewalle, 2005a, 2005bKane & Engle, 2000). Therefore, a smaller subset of learned effects (only the most spontaneously activated ones) is available to influence behavior.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%