2016
DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12453
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Wordform Similarity Increases With Semantic Similarity: An Analysis of 100 Languages

Abstract: Although the mapping between form and meaning is often regarded as arbitrary, there are in fact well-known constraints on words which are the result of functional pressures associated with language use and its acquisition. In particular, languages have been shown to encode meaning distinctions in their sound properties, which may be important for language learning. Here, we investigate the relationship between semantic distance and phonological distance in the largescale structure of the lexicon. We show evide… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
58
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 57 publications
(69 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
2
58
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This is probably driven by the differences in phonologies between languages, (or possibly due to iconic sound-meaning associations across languages, Blasi et al, 2016;; Dautriche et al, 2016), raising the possibility that the question words are not special. However, the E f for question words is lower than for the other sets and the z-values are twice as extreme, as can be seen in figure 4 which shows the comparisons of Ef to the permuted distributions.…”
Section: Samplementioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is probably driven by the differences in phonologies between languages, (or possibly due to iconic sound-meaning associations across languages, Blasi et al, 2016;; Dautriche et al, 2016), raising the possibility that the question words are not special. However, the E f for question words is lower than for the other sets and the z-values are twice as extreme, as can be seen in figure 4 which shows the comparisons of Ef to the permuted distributions.…”
Section: Samplementioning
confidence: 99%
“…On the one hand, methods proposals and critiques accompanied by exploratory results (Dubossarsky et al, 2017;Frermann and Lapata, 2016;Gulordava and Baroni, 2011;Hamilton et al, 2016b;Jatowt and Duh, 2014;Kulkarni et al, 2015;Sagi et al, 2011;Schlechtweg et al, 2017;Wijaya and Yeniterzi, 2011). On the other, applications of these methods, usually with more specific linguistic questions in mind (Dautriche et al, 2016;Dubossarsky et al, 2016;Hamilton et al, 2016a;Perek, 2016;Rodda et al, 2016;Xu and Kemp, 2015). Notably, all of these approaches are, one way or another, based on (co-occurrence) frequencies of words, and as such naturally subject to sampling biases potentially introduced by uneven representation of topics and genres in a corpus.…”
Section: Previous Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Unlike Dautriche et al (2017), who draw lexicons from Wikipedia, or Otis and Sagi (2008), we directly use a phone string representation, rather than their proxy of using each language's orthography. This makes our work the first to quantify the interface between phones and meaning in a massively multilingual setting.…”
Section: Datasetsmentioning
confidence: 99%