2005
DOI: 10.1016/j.bandl.2004.10.010
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Word length effect in early reading and in developmental dyslexia

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

37
171
1
11

Year Published

2012
2012
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 206 publications
(220 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
37
171
1
11
Order By: Relevance
“…Words with high values in the PCA-derived form factor are longer and have fewer neighbors. The effect on oral reading latencies of word length measured in letters (Balota et al, 2004;Weekes, 1997;Ziegler et al, 2001;Zoccolotti et al, 2005), in which longer words elicit longer naming latencies, is conventionally argued to reflect the serial phonological encoding of graphemes (Weekes, 1997). Our observation of a curvilinear length effect replicates, to some extent, previous findings in other languages (Ferrand et al, 2010;New et al, 2006).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 80%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Words with high values in the PCA-derived form factor are longer and have fewer neighbors. The effect on oral reading latencies of word length measured in letters (Balota et al, 2004;Weekes, 1997;Ziegler et al, 2001;Zoccolotti et al, 2005), in which longer words elicit longer naming latencies, is conventionally argued to reflect the serial phonological encoding of graphemes (Weekes, 1997). Our observation of a curvilinear length effect replicates, to some extent, previous findings in other languages (Ferrand et al, 2010;New et al, 2006).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 80%
“…The effect on oral reading latencies of word length measured in letters has been routinely observed in word naming in transparent orthography languages: in German (Ziegler, Perry, Jacobs, & Braun, 2001), in Italian (Barca et al, 2002;Burani et al, 2007;De Luca, Barca, Burani, & Zoccolotti, 2008;Zoccolotti et al, 2005), and in Spanish (Cuetos & Barbón, 2006). Recent studies have indicated that syllable length also affects performance in visual word recognition (New, Ferrand, Pallier, & Brysbaert, 2006; see also Butler & Hains, 1979) and in word naming (Jared & Seidenberg, 1990;Yap & Balota, 2009;see also Ferrand, 2000).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In transparent orthographies the main characteristic of readers with RD is slow reading (e.g., de Jong & van der Leij, 2003;Landerl & Wimmer, 2008;Landerl, Wimmer, & Frith, 1997;Zoccolotti et al, 2005) while reading accuracy has been shown to be an easy skill to acquire (Aro & Wimmer, 2003;Seymour, Aro, & Erskine, 2003). Therefore, unlike previous studies on RD stability in English, we focus on reading fluency.…”
Section: Late-emerging and Resolving Dyslexia: A Follow-up Study Frommentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, for first and second graders, as well as children with dyslexia, length effects have been observed for words of two to five letters (e.g., Marinus & de Jong, 2010b;Ziegler, Perry, Ma-Wyatt, Ladner, & Schulte-Körne, 2003;Zoccolotti et al, 2005). For sixth through eighth graders, length affected naming latencies for words ranging from three to eight letters ).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The effects of neighbourhood size are of interest for two reasons. First, according to PDP models, neighbourhood size is a possible alternative explanation for length effects, yet previous studies on length effects in children have not controlled for neighbourhood size (e.g., Martens & de Jong, 2006;Spinelli et al, 2005;Zoccolotti et al, 2005). In addition, in a large data set of adult lexical decisions, it has been shown that controlling for neighbourhood size is important when studying length effects.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%