2008
DOI: 10.1177/0023830908099070
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Word Length and Lexical Competition: Longer is the Same as Shorter

Abstract: Neighborhood density refers to the number of words that sound similar to a given word. Previous studies have found that neighborhood density influences the recognition of spoken words (Luce & Pisoni, 1998); however, this work has focused almost exclusively on monosyllabic words in English. To investigate the effects of neighborhood density on longer words, bisyllabic words varying in neighborhood density were presented auditorily to participants in a perceptual identification task and a lexical decision task. … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

2
25
1

Year Published

2009
2009
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

4
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(30 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
2
25
1
Order By: Relevance
“…We further grant that much has been learned by studying how the variables that contribute to those relationships influence processing. Consider all of the work investigating the influence of word frequency on various language and memory processes (dating back at least to Lester, 1922), studies of word-length on processing (e.g., Vitevitch, Stamer, & Sereno, 2008), all of the work on orthographic and phonological neighborhood density on processing (e.g., Laxon, Coltheart, & Keating, 1988; Pisoni, Nusbaum, Luce, & Slowiaczek, 1985), and the work on neighborhood frequency on processing (e.g., Grainger, O’Regan, Jacobs, & Segui, 1989). What appears to be lacking are studies that demonstrate that relationships between variables—such as the observation that high frequency words tend to have many phonological neighbors (e.g., Frauenfelder et al, 1993; Landauer & Streeter, 1973)—directly influence processing.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We further grant that much has been learned by studying how the variables that contribute to those relationships influence processing. Consider all of the work investigating the influence of word frequency on various language and memory processes (dating back at least to Lester, 1922), studies of word-length on processing (e.g., Vitevitch, Stamer, & Sereno, 2008), all of the work on orthographic and phonological neighborhood density on processing (e.g., Laxon, Coltheart, & Keating, 1988; Pisoni, Nusbaum, Luce, & Slowiaczek, 1985), and the work on neighborhood frequency on processing (e.g., Grainger, O’Regan, Jacobs, & Segui, 1989). What appears to be lacking are studies that demonstrate that relationships between variables—such as the observation that high frequency words tend to have many phonological neighbors (e.g., Frauenfelder et al, 1993; Landauer & Streeter, 1973)—directly influence processing.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, addressing this issue in the typical way, by using small sets of tightly controlled stimuli (e.g. Garlock, Walley, & Metsala, 2001; Mirman, Kittredge, & Dell, 2010; Vitevitch, 2002a, 2003; Vitevitch et al, 2004; Vitevitch & Rodriguez, 2005; Vitevitch & Stamer, 2006; Vitevitch, Stamer, & Sereno, 2008) may carry its own problems, as this accentuates the impact of item-specific stimulus characteristics, and may reduce the replicability of the findings (cf. Baus et al, 2008; Spieler & Balota, 2000; Vitevitch & Stamer, 2006).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…, each word has additional neighbors, but only a few were listed for illustrative purposes). Much psycholinguistic research has demonstrated the influence of neighborhood density on spoken word recognition (e.g., Cluff & Luce, 1990; Goldinger, Luce, & Pisoni, 1989; Luce & Pisoni, 1998; Vitevitch, 2002a, 2003; Vitevitch & Luce, 1999; Vitevitch & Rodríguez, 2005; Vitevitch, Stamer & Sereno, in press) and other language processes (e.g., Storkel, Armbruster & Hogan, 2006; Vitevitch, 1997, 2002b; Vitevitch & Stamer, 2006). …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%