2019
DOI: 10.1037/dec0000087
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Why we should quit while we’re ahead: When do averages matter more than sums?

Abstract: An enduring debate in decision-making and social cognition concerns the algorithm governing the formation of intuitive preferences and attitudes. Here we contrast 2 principles that are considered central to such judgments: averaging versus summation. Participants in 4 experiments were prompted to rely on their intuition when rating the Hall of Fame eligibility of basketball players, or their liking of athletes, lecturers or slot-machines, on the basis of rapid numerical sequences that represent performances, c… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

1
22
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

4
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(30 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
1
22
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In three experiments we examined the ability of human participants in a task of averaging of numerosity stimuli (sets of dots). This extends the range of operations on which numerosity representations were used from comparisons, addition or subtractions on pairs of stimuli, to the averaging of multiple stimulian operation that is of key importance to decision-making (Brusovansky et al, 2017;Vanunu et al, 2018;Weber, 2010). This task also extends the domain of stimuli on which the extraction of summary statistics was established, from domains such as size, orientation, emotional expression or object category (Ariely, 2001;Chong & Treisman, 2005;Dakin, 2001;Habrman et al, 2009;Haberman & Whitney, 2011;Khayat & Hochstein, 2018Parkes et al, 2001;Robitaille & Harris, 2011) to the domain of nonsymbolic numerosities across temporal sequences.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 95%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In three experiments we examined the ability of human participants in a task of averaging of numerosity stimuli (sets of dots). This extends the range of operations on which numerosity representations were used from comparisons, addition or subtractions on pairs of stimuli, to the averaging of multiple stimulian operation that is of key importance to decision-making (Brusovansky et al, 2017;Vanunu et al, 2018;Weber, 2010). This task also extends the domain of stimuli on which the extraction of summary statistics was established, from domains such as size, orientation, emotional expression or object category (Ariely, 2001;Chong & Treisman, 2005;Dakin, 2001;Habrman et al, 2009;Haberman & Whitney, 2011;Khayat & Hochstein, 2018Parkes et al, 2001;Robitaille & Harris, 2011) to the domain of nonsymbolic numerosities across temporal sequences.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…This is important for several reasons. First the estimation of the average is critical for common life activities, like decision-making, in which one has to estimate the utility of alternatives that vary across time or attributes (Betsch, Kaufmann, Lindow, Plessner, & Hoffmann, 2006;Brusovansky, Glickman, & Usher, 2018;Brusovansky, Vanunu, & Usher, 2017;Pleskac, Yu, Hopwood, & Liu, 2019;Roe, Busemeyer, & Townsend, 2001;Spitzer, Waschke, & Summerfield, 2017;Tsetsos, Chater, & Usher, 2012;Usher & McClelland, 2004;Vanunu, Pachur, & Usher, 2018;Zeigenfuse, Pleskac, & Liu, 2014). Second, recent research has indicated an impressive ability of human subjects in estimating summary statistics (in particular the average) of perceptual properties of sets of elements, such as size, orientation, and even emotional expression (Ariely, 2001;Chong & Treisman, 2005;Dakin, 2001;Haberman, Harp & Whitney, 2009;Haberman & Whitney, 2011;Khayat & Hochstein, 2018;Parkes, Lund, Angelucci, Solomon, & Morgan, 2001;Robitaille & Harris, 2011).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, Betsch and colleagues (2001) have demonstrated that human observers formed accurate attitudes towards alternatives (stocks—characterised as sequences of returns), which were presented as distractors and which the observers had no explicit goal to evaluate (see also Betsch, Kaufmann, Lindow, Plessner, & Hoffmann, 2006). More recently, Brusovansky, Vanunu, and Usher (2017) have shown that participants evaluated the subjective-value (attractiveness) of rapid sequences of numbers (of variable length, presented at two items per second, exceeding the capacity for symbolic computations 1 ), which represented athletes competing in an athletics contest, in a way that reflected the mean of the numbers in the sequence. Critically, participants were not able to discount deviant numbers that were enclosed in a salient red frame and that were said to represent “computer-errors,” although they expressed confidence that they did.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A central question that was subject to some debate is the principle that drives such intuitive preferences. While some studies (Betsch et al, 2006; Betsch et al, 2001) suggested that value integration follows a summative principle, others showed results consistent with an averaging principle (Anderson, 1981; Brusovansky et al, 2017; Kahneman, 2011). 2 For example, Brusovansky and colleagues (2017) demonstrated a so-called “Jordan-effect” whereby participants gave higher ratings (to indicate “Hall of Fame” potential) to sequences (corresponding to careers of basketball players) with a high mean, compared with the same sequences to which few extra numbers were added (i.e., few extra seasons, increasing the sum but reducing the mean).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation