1987
DOI: 10.1016/0028-3932(87)90067-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Why is there a left side underestimation in rod bisection?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
39
0

Year Published

1987
1987
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 110 publications
(47 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
6
39
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Indeed, leftward MI bias increased for trials starting on the left as compared with trials starting on the right. This is consistent with prior studies investigating the effects of directional scanning on line bisection, in which responses shifted toward the scanning startside (Bradshaw, Nathan, Nettleton, Wilson, & Pierson, 1987;Chokron, Bartolomeo, Perenin, Helft, & Imbert, 1998). An important consideration for the current study, however, is that the direction of scanning eye movements was dissociated from the direction of laser pointer movement in half of the trials.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…Indeed, leftward MI bias increased for trials starting on the left as compared with trials starting on the right. This is consistent with prior studies investigating the effects of directional scanning on line bisection, in which responses shifted toward the scanning startside (Bradshaw, Nathan, Nettleton, Wilson, & Pierson, 1987;Chokron, Bartolomeo, Perenin, Helft, & Imbert, 1998). An important consideration for the current study, however, is that the direction of scanning eye movements was dissociated from the direction of laser pointer movement in half of the trials.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…This advantage is proposed to be equivalent to the one enjoyed by left-sided items in a horizontal arrangement of visually or haptically presented stimuli [Bradshaw, Nathan, Nettleton, Wilson, & Pierson (1987): rod centering] and referred to, alternatively, as "initial exploration asymmetry" (Ebersbach et al, 1996;Hättig, 1992), "left-side underestimation" (Bradshaw, Nettleton, Nathan, & Wilson, 1983), "right hemispatial inattention" (Weintraub & Mesulam, 1988) or "pseudoneglect" (Bowers & Heilman, 1980). Pseudoneglect along the mental number line was originally demonstrated in the bisection of numerical intervals (Oliveri et al, 2004).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…No dierences were found between the left-to-right scan, the right-to-left scan, and a control condition in which subjects bisected lines without ®rst viewing them traced by the experimenter. Bradshaw et al [14] had subjects bisect a line by sliding it in a channel, with the subjects perceived line midpoint indicated by a stationary midpoint marker. In one condition subjects were asked to ®xate on the marker placed at line midpoint, while in the other condition subjects were allowed free visual scanning.…”
Section: Directional Scanningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Bradshaw et al [14] varied the salience of the rods which subjects bisected by using black or white rods on either matching backgrounds (i.e., black on black, low salience) or opposite backgrounds (i.e., white on black, high salience). Subjects erred to the left of veridical center across all experimental conditions, although when the left half of a rod was low in salience compared to the right, subjects erred farther to the left of veridical.…”
Section: Line Saliencementioning
confidence: 99%