2014
DOI: 10.3758/s13421-014-0454-6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Why does guessing incorrectly enhance, rather than impair, retention?

Abstract: The finding that trying, and failing, to predict the upcoming to-be-remembered response to a given cue can enhance later recall of that response, relative to studying the intact cue-response pair, is surprising, especially given that the standard paradigm (e.g., Kornell, Hays, & Bjork, 2009) involves allocating what would otherwise be study time to generating an error. In three experiments, we sought to eliminate two potential heuristics that participants might use to aid recall of correct responses on the fin… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
35
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 36 publications
(38 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
(51 reference statements)
3
35
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, while the total duration of the trials in each encoding condition was matched, the target was presented for longer in the Study condition than in the Meaning and First Word conditions. We chose to match the overall trial duration of the encoding conditions rather than the target presentation alone to be consistent with both Potts and Shanks' experiments and many others on the topic of unsuccessful retrieval (e.g., Knight et al, 2012;Yan et al, 2014;Yang et al, 2017). Indeed, Potts and Shanks' results are especially interesting precisely because errorful generation was beneficial even though it was at the expense of study time.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 89%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Thus, while the total duration of the trials in each encoding condition was matched, the target was presented for longer in the Study condition than in the Meaning and First Word conditions. We chose to match the overall trial duration of the encoding conditions rather than the target presentation alone to be consistent with both Potts and Shanks' experiments and many others on the topic of unsuccessful retrieval (e.g., Knight et al, 2012;Yan et al, 2014;Yang et al, 2017). Indeed, Potts and Shanks' results are especially interesting precisely because errorful generation was beneficial even though it was at the expense of study time.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…First, generating errors improved cued recall even when there was less time to encode the correct target in the test condition than in the study condition. Second, there was more scope for incorrect guesses to have interfered with the participants' memory of the targets (Huelser & Metcalfe, 2012;Yan, Yu, Garcia, & Bjork, 2014). Thus, the benefits of generating errors appeared to outweigh the costs of less study time and more interference.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…That such generation attempts, even when they are assured of being incorrect, can improve subsequent memory for the correct response has drawn a great deal of recent interest (Grimaldi & Karpicke, 2012; Hays, Kornell, & Bjork, 2013; Huelser & Metcalfe, 2012; Knight, Ball, Brewer, DeWitt, & Marsh, 2012; Kornell, Hays, & Bjork, 2009; Potts & Shanks, 2014; Yan, Yu, Garcia, & Bjork, 2014). An experimental paradigm introduced by Kornell et al (2009) has triggered much of this interest.…”
Section: Errorless Versus Errorful Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Support for this view comes from studies showing that generating errors during learning sometimes impairs memory on subsequent tests (Baddeley & Wilson, 1994;Forlano & Hoffman, 1937;Kessels & De Haan, 2003;Squires, Hunkin, & Parkin, 1997). The more frequent finding in recent years, however, is that errors aid learning (Cyr & Anderson, 2015;Grimaldi & Karpicke, 2012;Hays, Kornell, & Bjork, 2013;Huelser & Metcalfe, 2012;Kane & Anderson, 1978;Knight, Ball, Brewer, DeWitt, & Marsh, 2012;Kornell, 2014;Kornell, Hays, & Bjork, 2009;Richland, Kornell, & Kao, 2009;Slamecka & Fevreiski, 1983;Tanaka, Miyatani, & Iwaki, 2019;Vaughn & Rawson, 2012;Yan, Yu, Garcia, & Bjork, 2014;Yang, Potts, & Shanks, 2017;Zawadzka & Hanczakowski, 2018). In these cases, failed tests are beneficial, and they can even be as beneficial as successful tests (Kornell, Jacobs Klein, & Rawson, 2015).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%