2019
DOI: 10.1002/dev.21851
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Who can speak that language? Eleven‐month‐old infants have language‐dependent expectations regarding speaker ethnicity

Abstract: Research demonstrates that young infants attend to the indexical characteristics of speakers, including age, gender, and ethnicity, and that the relationship between language and ethnicity is intuitive among older children. However, little research has examined whether infants, within the first year, are sensitive to the co‐occurrences of ethnicity and language. In this paper, we demonstrate that by 11 months of age, infants hold language‐dependent expectations regarding speaker ethnicity. Specifically, 11‐mon… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
13
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
2
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Toddlers in this study had very little exposure to black individuals, but overall had some exposure to other racial backgrounds. In the context of a race for which they had a great deal of experience, infants had a strong linguistic expectation for this speaker, and mutual exclusivity was invoked (consistent with May et al, 2019; block 1 of Weatherhead & White, 2018). This result is consistent with monolinguals and bilinguals' prior experiences with familiar-race speakers, in that the label "zabe" was inconsistent with their known category labels for dog and cat.…”
Section: Remaining Questionssupporting
confidence: 61%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Toddlers in this study had very little exposure to black individuals, but overall had some exposure to other racial backgrounds. In the context of a race for which they had a great deal of experience, infants had a strong linguistic expectation for this speaker, and mutual exclusivity was invoked (consistent with May et al, 2019; block 1 of Weatherhead & White, 2018). This result is consistent with monolinguals and bilinguals' prior experiences with familiar-race speakers, in that the label "zabe" was inconsistent with their known category labels for dog and cat.…”
Section: Remaining Questionssupporting
confidence: 61%
“…For example, 6‐month‐olds were reported to match unfamiliar‐race faces with non‐native languages (Uttley et al., 2013), thereby demonstrating understanding of familiar‐race and unfamiliar‐race speakers as separate groups, and different beliefs about the languages these groups speak. By 11 months, infants in a community with many Chinese and English speakers had learned the relation between race and language, as illustrated by longer looking to East Asian faces when presented with a Chinese language than when presented with English, but comparable looking to East Asian and White faces when presented with an unfamiliar language (Spanish; May, Baron, & Werker, 2019). By 16 months, a speaker’s race influences recognition of familiar and unfamiliar pronunciations (e.g., “dog” vs. “dag”) of familiar words (Weatherhead & White, 2018).…”
Section: Current Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This lack of an effect at the population-level suggests that listeners are prepared for an Asian individual to speak the local accent, an expectation that is well-suited to the multi-ethnic landscape of their speech community. These results are in line with infants' developing expectations about the local native accent within this particular speech community [23], in addition to listeners' flexibility with "standard" German accents in the German-speaking context [24].…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…There is also evidence that infants take visual cues about a person into account when reasoning about what language an unfamiliar talker might use. At least by 11 months, monolingual English-exposed infants associated faces of East Asian women with hearing Cantonese (May et al, 2019), indicating that they use the specific language spoken as a cue to a person's identity in some cases. Further studies also show that monolingual infants take the speaker's race into account in language processing tasks (Singh et al, 2020;Weatherhead et al, 2021).…”
Section: Evidence For Sophisticated Reasoning About Language In Socia...mentioning
confidence: 99%