2010
DOI: 10.3758/mc.38.3.255
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

When two is too many: Collaborative encoding impairs memory

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

14
72
0
5

Year Published

2011
2011
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 90 publications
(91 citation statements)
references
References 70 publications
(74 reference statements)
14
72
0
5
Order By: Relevance
“…Among these are retrieval conditions where the retrieval task precludes retrieval disruption, as in the case of a recognition task in which all studied items are presented (Clark, Abbe, & Larson, 2006;Clark, Hori, Putnam, & Martin, 2000;Meade & Roediger, 2009) and in the case of a cued recall task in which the retrieval order is preset by the presentation order of the recall cues (Barber, Rajaram, & Aron, 2010;Finlay et al, 2000) and, therefore, retrieval disruption occurs to the same extent for both collaborative and nominal groups. Similarly, collaborative inhibition is also eliminated when participants are prevented from switching categories by requiring them to recall items from a given category contiguously before moving on to recalling items from another category (Basden et al, 1997, Experiment 4), once again aligning the same retrieval strategy for all participants.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Among these are retrieval conditions where the retrieval task precludes retrieval disruption, as in the case of a recognition task in which all studied items are presented (Clark, Abbe, & Larson, 2006;Clark, Hori, Putnam, & Martin, 2000;Meade & Roediger, 2009) and in the case of a cued recall task in which the retrieval order is preset by the presentation order of the recall cues (Barber, Rajaram, & Aron, 2010;Finlay et al, 2000) and, therefore, retrieval disruption occurs to the same extent for both collaborative and nominal groups. Similarly, collaborative inhibition is also eliminated when participants are prevented from switching categories by requiring them to recall items from a given category contiguously before moving on to recalling items from another category (Basden et al, 1997, Experiment 4), once again aligning the same retrieval strategy for all participants.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast with Andersson and Rönn-berg (1995), Finlay, Hitch, and Meudell (2000) found that participants who engaged in a shared incidental encoding where they jointly searched for target animals in pictures subsequently showed no collaborative inhibition. Finally, Barber, Rajaram, and Aron (2010) found that individuals who engaged in a collaborative encoding task, where they took turns to generate sentences containing target words, subsequently performed more poorly on an individual recall test. However, they did not examine the outcomes in terms of the costs and benefits for collaborative recall.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Mario lo había visitado años antes en su primera visita a París mientras que Liliana lo visitó por primera vez en sus últimas vacaciones con Mario). En consecuencia, la información que Liliana y Mario recuerdan acerca de su visita al Centro George-Pompidou de forma colaborativa puede ser el resultado de haber codificado una misma o similar experiencia de forma compartida o separada (Barber, Rajaram, & Aron, 2010;Barnier, Sutton, Harris, & Wilson, 2008;Harris, Barnier, & Sutton, 2013;Pereira-Pasarin & Rajaram, 2011). Por supuesto que nuestras memorias individuales y colaborativas no siempre representan de forma acertada los acontecimientos que ocurrieron en el pasado, más bien funcionan como representaciones individuales y compartidas de ese pasado (Sutton, 2008).…”
Section: Hse -Social and Education History 7(2) 127unclassified