2001
DOI: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1799
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

When should a territory resident attack?

Abstract: Models of territorial defence tend to omit two characteristics of many territorial systems: repeated intrusions by the same individual and the learning processes of residents and intruders. Here we present state-dependent, dynamic models of feeding territories, designed to investigate temporal patterns of resident aggression towards intruders that are capable of spatial learning. We compare two types of models: (1) a nomadic intruder model, in which intruders never visit the same territory twice, and (2) a sin… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
26
0

Year Published

2003
2003
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
3
2
2

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 42 publications
(29 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
3
26
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Levels of aggression in contests between familiar individuals are lower than in contests between strangers, so that familiarity appears to reduce aggression and increase the foraging rate (Griffiths et al 2004). This is consistent with the hypothesis that individual recognition is used to reduce the costs of contesting resources: After an initial contest, encounters between familiar individuals should be settled with less aggression and a lower probability of status reversal than encounters between unfamiliar fish (Switzer et al 2001). However, this effect decays rapidly with time since previously familiar individuals are treated as strangers after 3 days of separation (Johnsson 1997).…”
Section: Competitionsupporting
confidence: 72%
“…Levels of aggression in contests between familiar individuals are lower than in contests between strangers, so that familiarity appears to reduce aggression and increase the foraging rate (Griffiths et al 2004). This is consistent with the hypothesis that individual recognition is used to reduce the costs of contesting resources: After an initial contest, encounters between familiar individuals should be settled with less aggression and a lower probability of status reversal than encounters between unfamiliar fish (Switzer et al 2001). However, this effect decays rapidly with time since previously familiar individuals are treated as strangers after 3 days of separation (Johnsson 1997).…”
Section: Competitionsupporting
confidence: 72%
“…However, niche-construction can also occur in many other situations, notably including social environments that are generated and maintained as a result of the behaviour of a focal individual. For instance, when aggressive behaviour by territorial animals discourages conspecifics from returning to the area where they were attacked, a focal individual who was highly aggressive at the time of territory establishment would have lower ongoing rates of social interactions for the rest of that season than a focal individual who was less aggressive during that same period (Stamps & Krishnan 2001;Switzer et al 2001).…”
Section: Genes Experience and Contextual Generalitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…[34,35]). Specific aspects of the context the conflict happens in can, however, also be important, such as the likelihood of a repeated conflict [36], or the amount of knowledge an individual has about the opponents' strategy [37,38].…”
Section: Different Kinds Of Conflictsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, important aspects such as asymmetry of information between opponents [37], likelihood of a repeated conflict [36] and controllability of access [34] all vary dependent on which of these types of access to the territory individuals compete for. Each of these types of conflicts therefore has its own specific properties with specific evolutionary consequences for the individuals' behaviour.…”
Section: Different Kinds Of Conflictsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation