2018
DOI: 10.7287/peerj.preprints.27026v1
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

When and where to hatch? Red-eyed treefrog embryos use light cues in two contexts

Abstract: Hatching timing is under strong selection and environmentally cued in many species.Embryos use multiple sensory modalities to inform hatching timing and many have spontaneous hatching patterns adaptively synchronized to natural cycles. Embryos can also adaptively shift their hatching timing in response to environmental cues indicating immediate threats or opportunities. Such cued shifts in hatching are widespread among amphibians; however, we know little about what, if anything, regulates their spontaneous hat… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
2

Relationship

1
1

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 2 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…They might also behaviorally facilitate rupture by pressing their head against the membrane to increase HGC–membrane contact. Flooded embryos lose the oxygen gradient that helps them orient—and hatch—toward the exposed side of their egg, and in glass cups they also lose directional light cues; this increases the frequency of hatching complications and need for a second rupture site (Güell and Warkentin, 2018; Salazar-Nicholls et al, 2017). Moreover, in both snake and wasp attacks embryos sometimes move—or are pushed—away from their initial rupture site (KMW, observations from video recordings).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They might also behaviorally facilitate rupture by pressing their head against the membrane to increase HGC–membrane contact. Flooded embryos lose the oxygen gradient that helps them orient—and hatch—toward the exposed side of their egg, and in glass cups they also lose directional light cues; this increases the frequency of hatching complications and need for a second rupture site (Güell and Warkentin, 2018; Salazar-Nicholls et al, 2017). Moreover, in both snake and wasp attacks embryos sometimes move—or are pushed—away from their initial rupture site (KMW, observations from video recordings).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We tested the effect of gentamicin on jiggling-induced hatching in two later periods to determine if the dependence of MCH on lateral line function changes developmentally. In A. callidryas, embryos hatch spontaneously from 5-7 d, while younger embryos almost never hatch if undisturbed (Güell and Warkentin, 2018;Hite et al, 2018;Warkentin, 2000;Warkentin et al, 2001). From age 4 to 5 d, embryos become more likely to escape during snake and wasp attacks (Gomez-Mestre and Warkentin, 2007;Warkentin, 1995;Warkentin, 2000) and to hatch in vibration playbacks (Jung et al, 2019;Warkentin et al, In press;Warkentin et al, 2019).…”
Section: Hatching-response Test: Manual Egg Jigglingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The embryos of Agalychnis callidryas clearly use cues in multiple sensory modalities, including vibration (Warkentin 2005, hypoxia (Rogge and Warkentin, 2008) and light level (Güell and Warkentin, 2018), to inform hatching. In sensing physical disturbances, these embryos might use one or multiple mechanosensory systems, either to perceive different cue components available in attacks or as potentially redundant or synergistic sensors of the same cue component.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%