2016
DOI: 10.1353/lan.2016.0034
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

What sound symbolism can and cannot do: Testing the iconicity of ideophones from five languages

Abstract: Sound symbolism is a phenomenon with broad relevance to the study of language and mind, but there has been a disconnect between its investigations in linguistics and psychology. This study tests the sound-symbolic potential of ideophones-words described as iconic-in an experimental task that improves over prior work in terms of ecological validity and experimental control. We presented 203 ideophones from five languages to eighty-two Dutch listeners in a binary-choice task, in four versions: original recording… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3

Citation Types

9
107
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 111 publications
(122 citation statements)
references
References 59 publications
(56 reference statements)
9
107
1
Order By: Relevance
“…African languages and many others use a lot of ideophones, "marked words that depict sensory imagery" (Dingemanse 2012: 655 andcf. Dingemanse 2011;Childs 1994Childs , 2015Voeltz & Kilian-Hatz 2001;Dingemanse et al 2016). Japanese dictionaries list around 4.500 sound symbolic words (Imai & Kita 2014;Oszmiańska 2001).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…African languages and many others use a lot of ideophones, "marked words that depict sensory imagery" (Dingemanse 2012: 655 andcf. Dingemanse 2011;Childs 1994Childs , 2015Voeltz & Kilian-Hatz 2001;Dingemanse et al 2016). Japanese dictionaries list around 4.500 sound symbolic words (Imai & Kita 2014;Oszmiańska 2001).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, some authors have speculated that pairing sounds with congruent meanings in real language may serve to highlight potential associations (e.g., Waugh, 1993). Dingemanse et al (2016) point out that in some cases it is necessary to know the definition of a word in order to appreciate the sound symbolic association between its phonemes and meaning. That is, would one appreciate the sound symbolism of goro without knowing that its definition related to heaviness?…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Being inherently expressive, ideophones provide the best locus to study iconicity (i.e., a perceptual resemblance between form and meaning) in spoken language (Dingemanse et al, 2016;Perry et al, 2015). Recent empirical support for the iconicity of ideophones is found in Dingemanse et al's (2016) study, where native Dutch speakers matched the meanings of existing ideophones (from five ideophone-rich languages that they did not speak) above the chance level of 50%. Cross-linguistically, such (canonically) iconic words often differentiate themselves from ordinary vocabulary by means of skewed phonotactic distributions (Childs, 2014;Dingemanse, 2012;Dingemanse et al, 2015).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…2 The twofold onomatopoeic/cross-modal distinction is based on the traditional semantic categorization of the Korean ideophonic lexicon, i.e., ɨjsəŋə 'phonomimes' for depiction of auditory experiences and ɨjtɛə 'phenomimes plus psychomimes' for depiction of non-auditory experiences, such as visual or tactile sensations or psychological states. A more fine-grained level of distinction is possible, for example, by subdividing cross-modal domains into motion, texture, shape, and visual appearance, as in Dingemanse et al (2016). However, the principal aim of this paper is to examine whether ideophones of the highest iconicity display the greatest diversity in the regular phonotactic of the ideophonic system.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation