Our system is currently under heavy load due to increased usage. We're actively working on upgrades to improve performance. Thank you for your patience.
1983
DOI: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90012-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

What some concepts might not be

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

6
295
0
16

Year Published

1998
1998
2006
2006

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 811 publications
(321 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
6
295
0
16
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, it is now unclear whether the early research has any bearing whatsoever on conclusions about category structure (e.g., Armstrong et al, 1983). Since this realization, few studies have been conducted to directly investigate whether semantic categories have absolute or graded structure.…”
Section: Evidence Of Category Structurementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Thus, it is now unclear whether the early research has any bearing whatsoever on conclusions about category structure (e.g., Armstrong et al, 1983). Since this realization, few studies have been conducted to directly investigate whether semantic categories have absolute or graded structure.…”
Section: Evidence Of Category Structurementioning
confidence: 99%
“…that some members of semantic categories have greater membership status than do other, less typical members (but, for alternative interpretations, see Armstrong, Gleitman, & Gleitman, 1983;Bourne, 1982;Landau, 1982). Furthermore, an overall high positive correlation between typicality and categorization is observed in most semantic categories (see, e.g., Diesendruck & Gelman, 1999;Hampton, 1998;McCloskey & Glucksberg, 1978;Rosch & Mervis, 1975).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One can generate pairs of extreme situations, such that just about everyone would agree that categorization is based on one strategy in one case and on a different procedure in the other case. Thus, if a person has to categorize two-digit numbers as odd or even, presumably all researchers would agree that the categorizer does it by applying the rule of 'Divisible by 2 or not' (Armstrong et al, 1983); but if that same person has to categorize novel people with respect to whether they are as friendly as the neighbors on their block, presumably most would agree that the categorizer now relies on memories of his or her neighbors (Kahneman and Miller, 1986). Note, however, an important aspect of this counterexample-the different putative categorization procedures are applied to different kinds of categories, where one category is part of a rule-based formal system and the other one is completely ad hoc.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Reviewing arguments from Goodman (1970), Osherson and Smith (1982), Armstrong et al (1983), Murphy and Medin (1985), and others, Rips made the case that membership in natural categories was not primarily dependent on similarity. The argument is that the way in which category membership is determined is different from the way in which typicality is derived.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%