2008
DOI: 10.1080/17470210701508822
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

What Phonological Deficit?

Abstract: We review a series of experiments aimed at understanding the nature of the phonological deficit in developmental dyslexia. These experiments investigate input and output phonological representations, phonological grammar, foreign speech perception and production, and unconscious speech processing and lexical access. Our results converge on the observation that the phonological representations of people with dyslexia may be intact, and that the phonological deficit surfaces only as a function of certain task re… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

47
477
7
12

Year Published

2012
2012
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 510 publications
(543 citation statements)
references
References 56 publications
47
477
7
12
Order By: Relevance
“…Indeed, the more difficult phonological awareness tasks also appeal to children's working memory by demanding more updating capacities. This is in line with the suggestion by Ramus and Szenkovits (2008) who argued that the phonological representations of people with dyslexia may be intact, but not their access to these representations. They thus suggest that the phonological deficit can be seen as a function of task requirements.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…Indeed, the more difficult phonological awareness tasks also appeal to children's working memory by demanding more updating capacities. This is in line with the suggestion by Ramus and Szenkovits (2008) who argued that the phonological representations of people with dyslexia may be intact, but not their access to these representations. They thus suggest that the phonological deficit can be seen as a function of task requirements.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…5 More recently, Roberts and McDougall (2003) distinguished slightly different categories, namely: implicit awareness (e.g., matching); production and discrimination (e.g., oddity, similarity detection); and manipulation (e.g., segmentation, blending). Yet another way of conceiving of the processing demands has been to treat the status of phonological speech representations separately from the metacognitive task demands such as short-term memory or conscious awareness that might be involved in accessing such representations (Ramus & Szenkovits, 2008). This acknowledges that even implicit phonological awareness tasks do not reflect speech representations as directly as online measures of speech processing since meta-cognitive processing is required and is likely to become increasingly involved with age and reading experience.…”
Section: Overviewmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is probably not the case for unfamiliar words for which they show reduced sensitivity, thereby treating unknown words at a less refined grain-size [6][7][8]. PhPS and refined phonological representations play an essential part in a large number of cognitive processes, such as lexical access, reading, spelling, and learning new vocabulary [9][10][11][12]. PhPS are in turn closely connected to the phonological storage and rehearsal capacity in working memory, and to the semantic and phonological lexicon [7,8,13].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We constructed a global measure of PhPS, i.e., a phonological composite score that reflects various aspects of phonological representations [10]. The phonological composite score enabled us to measure and compare DHH and NH children's general PhPS skills with respect to their different auditory experience.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation