2012
DOI: 10.1093/czoolo/58.5.698
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

What makes a cry a cry? A review of infant distress vocalizations

Abstract: In contrast to the cries of human infants, sounds made by non-human infants in different stressful behavioral contexts (hunger or physical discomfort, isolation, capture by humans or predators) are usually treated as distinct types of vocalizations. However, if distress vocalizations produced by different species and in different contexts share a common motivational state and associated neurochemical pathways, we can expect them to share a common acoustic structure and adaptive function, showing only limited v… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...

Citation Types

7
150
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 151 publications
(159 citation statements)
references
References 136 publications
7
150
1
Order By: Relevance
“…From the moment of birth, certain signals from babies effectively influence parenting: Infant cries motivate adults to approach and to act (7,8). That is, infant cries and caregiver responses to them constitute an integrated dyadic system that encompasses the infant production of cries as well as the adult anatomy (9-12), physiology (5, 13), and perception, processing, and response apparatus to cries (2,4,5,(14)(15)(16)(17). Cries put both infant and caregiver in states of strong mutual nervous system activation and increase the probability of behavioral attunement (18).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…From the moment of birth, certain signals from babies effectively influence parenting: Infant cries motivate adults to approach and to act (7,8). That is, infant cries and caregiver responses to them constitute an integrated dyadic system that encompasses the infant production of cries as well as the adult anatomy (9-12), physiology (5, 13), and perception, processing, and response apparatus to cries (2,4,5,(14)(15)(16)(17). Cries put both infant and caregiver in states of strong mutual nervous system activation and increase the probability of behavioral attunement (18).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In observations of maternal behavior in situ in 11 countries, we hypothesized preferred and common patterns of new mothers' responses to their own infants' cries. In three independent companion fMRI experiments in three countries, we sought to identify neurobiological underpinnings of the same behavioral responses in new mothers in the United States to their own young infants' cries, in experienced mothers in China to infant cries in comparison with noninfant cry emotional sounds, and in experienced mothers and inexperienced nonmothers in Italy to generic infant cries.Care-soliciting vocalizations are common in the young of many species (2,6,16,20,21) and constitute a signal system that has been conserved throughout mammalian evolution (2,22). Human infants effectively elicit parental attention, proximity, and solicitude by crying (23-29); in times of famine, for example, crying…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In fact, aside from laughter (see Bryant et al, 2016;Scott, Lavan, Chen, & McGettigan, 2014) and infant cries (Lingle, Wyman, Kotrba, Teichroeb, & Romanow, 2012 for review), human nonverbal vocalisations (such as moans, sighs, roars, screams, and grunts) have received little attention, especially from a functional and evolutionary perspective.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Human nonverbal vocalisations likely predate language, and appear homologous in structure and function with nonhuman vocalisations (e.g. laughter Davila-Ross, Owren, & Zimmermann, 2010;Pisanski, Cartei, McGettigan, Raine, & Reby, 2016; infant distress vocalisations Lingle et al, 2012). As such, they may constitute a relatively direct link between animal and human vocal systems.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%