2016
DOI: 10.3758/s13420-016-0228-z
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

What is timed in a fixed-interval temporal bisection procedure?

Abstract: Recent research on interval timing in the behavioral and neurological sciences has employed a concurrent fixedinterval (FI) procedure first reported by Platt and Davis (Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 9, 160-170, 1983). Studies employing the task typically assess just 1 dependent variable, the switch/bisection point; however, multiple measures of timing are available in the procedure and it is unclear (a) what is timed (i.e., learned) by subjects and (b) what other measures migh… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…To give examples, the seal's Weber fraction is comparable to the Weber fractions obtained in adult humans of different age for STI pairs of 2 s/8 s (Ortega & López, 2008) or 3 s/6 s (Lustig & Meck, 2011; Penney et al, 2000; Penney et al, 2014), however, the seal's timing performance is worse for an STI pair of 4 s/8 s (Zélanti & Droit‐Volet, 2011); comparing seals with children again changes the picture as the children's performance is far inferior when tested with STI pairs of 1 s/4 s and 2 s/8 s (Droit‐Volet et al, 2003; Droit‐Volet & Clement, 2001; Rattat & Droit‐Volet, 2001). Pigeons and seals perform comparably precise for an STI pair of 1 s/4 s, whereas seals are outperformed by pigeons for an STI pair of 2 s/8 s (Fox et al, 2016). Nevertheless, our bisection results further strengthen the overall impression obtained in previous seal timing studies (Heinrich et al, 2016; Heinrich et al, 2020) that timing in seals compares favorably with the timing performances of other species, and differences in performance can most likely be attributed to the fact that the studies differ in experimental details and not that the timing abilities differ systematically.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…To give examples, the seal's Weber fraction is comparable to the Weber fractions obtained in adult humans of different age for STI pairs of 2 s/8 s (Ortega & López, 2008) or 3 s/6 s (Lustig & Meck, 2011; Penney et al, 2000; Penney et al, 2014), however, the seal's timing performance is worse for an STI pair of 4 s/8 s (Zélanti & Droit‐Volet, 2011); comparing seals with children again changes the picture as the children's performance is far inferior when tested with STI pairs of 1 s/4 s and 2 s/8 s (Droit‐Volet et al, 2003; Droit‐Volet & Clement, 2001; Rattat & Droit‐Volet, 2001). Pigeons and seals perform comparably precise for an STI pair of 1 s/4 s, whereas seals are outperformed by pigeons for an STI pair of 2 s/8 s (Fox et al, 2016). Nevertheless, our bisection results further strengthen the overall impression obtained in previous seal timing studies (Heinrich et al, 2016; Heinrich et al, 2020) that timing in seals compares favorably with the timing performances of other species, and differences in performance can most likely be attributed to the fact that the studies differ in experimental details and not that the timing abilities differ systematically.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, these experiments determined how these two aspects change with the properties of the stimulus (Droit‐Volet, Meck, & Penney, 2007; Lustig & Meck, 2011; Meck, 1984; Ortega & López, 2008; Penney, Gibbon, & Meck, 2000), the duration of the STIs (Wearden et al, 1997), stimulus ratio (Wearden & Ferrara, 1996), or the different spacing of the TTIs (Penney, Gibbon, & Meck, 2008; Raslear, 1985; Wearden & Ferrara, 1995), just to name a few modifications. This way the timing abilities in a variety of organisms, such as rats (e.g., Church & Deluty, 1977; Kim, Ghim, Lee, & Jung, 2013; Meck, 1983, 1984; Raslear, 1985; Siegel & Church, 1984), pigeons (e.g., Araiba & Brown, 2017; Fox, Prue, & Kyonka, 2016; Laude, Daniels, Wade, & Zentall, 2016; Machado & Keen, 1999; Machado & Pata, 2005; Pinheiro de Carvalho, Machado, & Tonneau, 2016; Platt & Davis, 1983), mice (e.g., Akdoğan & Balcı, 2016; Penney et al, 2008), dogs (Domeniconi & Machado, 2017), and humans of different age (e.g., Allan, 1991; Allan & Gerhardt, 2001; Droit‐Volet, Clément, & Fayol, 2003; Droit‐Volet & Wearden, 2001; Kopec & Brody, 2010; Lustig & Meck, 2011; Provasi, Rattat, & Droit‐Volet, 2011; Rattat & Droit‐Volet, 2001; Wearden, 1991b; Wearden et al, 1997) were characterized.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In Experiment 1, some rats (SL-RI) were trained to initiate their trials and those of other rats (EI) in a switch-timing task. In this task (Fox, Prue, & Kyonka, 2016), there are two potential target responses: the latency-to-switch (LTS) to the later source of food (Long-FI; Daniels et al, 2015b), and the latency-to-depart (LTD) from the earlier source of food (e.g., Balci et al, 2008). After performance stabilized, rats were challenged with a 1-h pre-feeding probe.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…If non-human animals are uninterested in maximizing the informativeness of their responses, equation ( 5) should suffice for them. This has been shown to be the case: Dogs (Cliff et al, 2019;Domeniconi & Machado, 2017), pigeons (Araiba & Brown, 2017;Fetterman & Killeen, 1992;Fox et al, 2016;Platt & Davis, 1983) and rats (Church & Deluty, 1977;Kim et al, 2009;Orduña et al, 2007) bisect at or near the GM (Note 3).…”
Section: Other Speciesmentioning
confidence: 92%