2003
DOI: 10.1016/s0010-0277(03)00116-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

What infants know about syntax but couldn't have learned: experimental evidence for syntactic structure at 18 months

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
139
0
3

Year Published

2007
2007
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 163 publications
(149 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
5
139
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Nonetheless, the above findings provide substantial motivation for further developing usage-based accounts of language acquisition. This optimism is supported by demonstrations of the functional underpinnings of anaphora constraints (van Hoek, 1997;Harris and Bates 2002) and proposals that these need not be taken as a priori unlearnable (Akhtar et al 2004;Lidz et al 2003;;MacWhinney, 2004). The proposal put forward here is that children's knowledge of anaphora constraints depends on understanding i) the relative accessibility of di¤erent referring expressions ii) the contexts in which referring expressions occur, particularly in within-sentence grammatical hierarchies and iii) the contrastive values of pronouns and reflexives.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 79%
“…Nonetheless, the above findings provide substantial motivation for further developing usage-based accounts of language acquisition. This optimism is supported by demonstrations of the functional underpinnings of anaphora constraints (van Hoek, 1997;Harris and Bates 2002) and proposals that these need not be taken as a priori unlearnable (Akhtar et al 2004;Lidz et al 2003;;MacWhinney, 2004). The proposal put forward here is that children's knowledge of anaphora constraints depends on understanding i) the relative accessibility of di¤erent referring expressions ii) the contexts in which referring expressions occur, particularly in within-sentence grammatical hierarchies and iii) the contrastive values of pronouns and reflexives.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 79%
“…Oh, look -there's another black cat.) Lidz, Waxman, & Freedman (2003) ran a series of experiments with 18-month-old children to test their interpretations of one in utterances like (1), and found that they too shared this intuition. So, Lidz, Waxman, & Freedman (2003) concluded that this knowledge about how to interpret one must be known by 18 months.…”
Section: Syntax-semantics Mappingmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…These ambiguous data dominate children's input for anaphoric one, and only rarely do unambiguous data appear (approximately 0.25% according to a corpus analysis by Lidz, Waxman, & Freedman (2003) and never in the corpus analysis conducted by Pearl & Mis (2011)). This is unsurprising once we realize that unambiguous data require a specific coincidence of utterance and situation.…”
Section: Syntax-semantics Mappingmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…The central puzzle of language acquisition is to determine which features of the acquired abstract system are constructed on the basis of experience and which features require specialized knowledge structures that restrict the ways that children generalize. Typically, we see the influence of children's learning mechanisms through patterns of overgeneralization (28)(29)(30)(31)(32), patterns in which children amplify less-reliable statistical patterns (33)(34)(35), or patterns in which children's knowledge goes beyond what is expressed in their exposure (23,(36)(37)(38)(39)(40)(41). Here, we have found a previously unidentified source of evidence for the child's contribution to language learning: a pattern of unpredictable individual variation among speakers, including children and their own parents, of ostensibly a single language.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%