2012
DOI: 10.2139/ssrn.2071478
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

What Dilemma? Moral Evaluation Shapes Factual Belief

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
50
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 37 publications
(50 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
0
50
0
Order By: Relevance
“…with the responses coded as 1 through 4, respectively). We also employed Liu and Ditto's (2013) two measures of whether torture is morally right ("The use of forceful or harsh interrogation techniques on individuals suspected of terrorist activities is …," on a seven-point scale ranging from "Morally acceptable in most or all cases" to "Morally wrong in most or all cases," reverse coded to indicate moral acceptance) and deontologically right ("The use of […] is morally wrong even if it is effective in getting suspects to talk," rated on a seven-point scale ranging from "Strongly disagree" to "Strongly agree," reverse coded to indicate moral acceptance).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…with the responses coded as 1 through 4, respectively). We also employed Liu and Ditto's (2013) two measures of whether torture is morally right ("The use of forceful or harsh interrogation techniques on individuals suspected of terrorist activities is …," on a seven-point scale ranging from "Morally acceptable in most or all cases" to "Morally wrong in most or all cases," reverse coded to indicate moral acceptance) and deontologically right ("The use of […] is morally wrong even if it is effective in getting suspects to talk," rated on a seven-point scale ranging from "Strongly disagree" to "Strongly agree," reverse coded to indicate moral acceptance).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In scholarship on moral judgment, recent work argues that people readily use a "consequentialist crutch" to rationalize moral stances and achieve coherence in their perceptions: Those who see something as deontologically moral-as inherently right, consequences aside-also tend to see it as effective and beneficial (Ditto & Liu, 2011). Importantly, Liu and Ditto (2013) showed that those who deemed torture deontologically acceptable also believed it was generally effective. These results are consistent with the effect we posit, but leave open the possibility that broad expectations of torture's effectiveness cause general acceptance of torture.…”
Section: Background and Plan Of Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Robust support for predictions based on the person-centered account of moral judgment (Pizarro & Tannenbaum, 2011;Uhlmann et al, 2015) was obtained across six original findings. The replication results also supported predictions regarding moral coherence (Liu & Ditto, 2013;Clark et al, in press) in perceptions of economic variables, but did not find consistent support for two specific hypotheses concerning moral judgments of organizations. Thus, the PPIR process allowed categories of robust effects to be separated from categories of findings that were less robust.…”
Section: Holistic Assessment Of Replication Resultsmentioning
confidence: 53%
“…Such a correlation raises the possibility that moral concerns about fairness irrationally influence perceptions of economic processes. In other words, aspects of free markets that seem unfair on moral grounds (e.g., replacing hard-working factory workers with automated machinery that can do the job more cheaply) may be subject to distorted perceptions of their objective economic effects (a moral coherence effect; Clark, Chen, & Ditto, in press;Liu & Ditto, 2013).…”
Section: Morality and Marketsmentioning
confidence: 99%