2012
DOI: 10.1017/s0305000912000141
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

What counts as effective input for word learning?

Abstract: The talk children hear from their primary caregivers predicts the size of their vocabularies. But children who spend time with multiple individuals also hear talk that others direct to them, as well as talk not directed to them at all. We investigated the effect of linguistic input on vocabulary acquisition in children who routinely spent time with one vs. multiple individuals. For all children, the number of words primary caregivers directed to them at age 2;6 predicted vocabulary size at age 3;6. For childre… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

10
124
1
7

Year Published

2012
2012
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 151 publications
(142 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
(41 reference statements)
10
124
1
7
Order By: Relevance
“…The second area has to do with determining the different kinds of attentional and learning strategies and associated benefits that are cultivated when children are routinely exposed to different kinds of social configurations and normative language practices other than predominantly child‐directed speech (Brown & Gaskins, ). For example, when referring to recent studies that show relations between child‐directed speech and later vocabulary size but not for overheard speech and later vocabulary size (Shneidman & Goldin‐Meadow, ; Shneidman et al., ; Weisleder & Fernald, ), Brown and Gaskins added an important caveat:
However, these studies treat all speech not directly addressed to the child as “overheard,” ignoring the fact that much of the speech (e.g., of adults on the phone, or adult–adult conversations) is irrelevant to the child who may well not be actually “overhearing” it. Such studies need to have more sensitive assessments of what the child is potentially attending to (actually overhearing) and more subtle analysis of the target vocabulary set in the different settings, before this issue will be clarified.
…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The second area has to do with determining the different kinds of attentional and learning strategies and associated benefits that are cultivated when children are routinely exposed to different kinds of social configurations and normative language practices other than predominantly child‐directed speech (Brown & Gaskins, ). For example, when referring to recent studies that show relations between child‐directed speech and later vocabulary size but not for overheard speech and later vocabulary size (Shneidman & Goldin‐Meadow, ; Shneidman et al., ; Weisleder & Fernald, ), Brown and Gaskins added an important caveat:
However, these studies treat all speech not directly addressed to the child as “overheard,” ignoring the fact that much of the speech (e.g., of adults on the phone, or adult–adult conversations) is irrelevant to the child who may well not be actually “overhearing” it. Such studies need to have more sensitive assessments of what the child is potentially attending to (actually overhearing) and more subtle analysis of the target vocabulary set in the different settings, before this issue will be clarified.
…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It also shows that speech directed to children predicts later vocabulary growth and language outcomes in school and suggests that different features of directed speech may have different effects as children get older. Although several studies have examined relationships between overheard versus directed speech and children's later vocabulary knowledge, and have shown a correlation with directed speech only (Shneidman & Goldin‐Meadow, ; Shneidman et al., ; Weisleder & Fernald, ), this literature tells us very little about overheard speech—its frequency in different sociocultural groups, its features, or its possible benefits. These issues are not trivial, particularly because one of the most robust findings from cross‐cultural research is the prevalence and efficacy of observational learning across a host of developmental domains (e.g., Gaskins & Paradise, ; Lancy, ; Miller & Cho, ; Ochs & Schieffelin, ; Rogoff, Paradise, Arauz, Correa‐Chávez, & Angelillo, ).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We included children as input talkers due to emerging evidence that infants preferably listen to children's voices and presumably learn from them as well (Polka, Masapollo, & Ménard, ). Further, Shneidman, Arroyo, Levine, and Goldin‐Meadow () showed that all speech directed to children, including from their siblings (and presumably other children), predicts vocabulary development better than input from the main caregiver alone (but see Shneidman & Goldin‐Meadow, ; for conflicting results with Mayan children). The number of children in infants’ input was comparatively low, with 79 of 138 participants not hearing speech from children at all.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We included children as input talkers due to emerging evidence that infants preferably listen to children's voices and presumably learn from them as well (Polka, Masapollo, & M enard, 2014). Further, Shneidman, Arroyo, Levine, and Goldin-Meadow (2013) showed that all speech directed to children, including from their siblings (and presumably other children), predicts vocabulary development better TALKER NUMBER AND NATIVE VOWEL DISCRIMINATION than input from the main caregiver alone (but see Shneidman & Goldin-Meadow, 2012; for conflicting results with Mayan children). The number of children in infants' input was comparatively low, with 79 of 138 participants not hearing speech from children at all.…”
Section: Questionnairementioning
confidence: 99%