2022
DOI: 10.1037/dev0001294
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

What could have been done? Counterfactual alternatives to negative outcomes generated by religious and secular children.

Abstract: Recent research has shown that a religious upbringing renders children receptive to ordinarily impossible outcomes, but the underlying mechanism for this effect remains unclear. Exposure to religious teachings might alter children’s basic understanding of causality. Alternatively, religious exposure might only affect children’s religious cognition, not their causal judgments more generally. To test between these possibilities, 6- to 11-year-old children attending either secular (n = 49, 51% female, primarily W… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
3
1
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 58 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Children also demonstrate an increased belief that prayer will work between the ages of 4 and 8 (Woolley and Phelps 2001). Children between the ages of 6 and 11 from varied religious backgrounds in the United States indicate prayer would be more effective than wishing or magic to prevent an unwanted, negative outcome from occurring (Payir et al 2022). However, between the ages of 3 and 11, children from varied religious backgrounds in the United States are also more likely to claim that events that are probable are more likely to occur through prayer than events that are physically impossible (Lane 2020).…”
Section: Asking God For Help: Children's Views On What To Pray For Whenmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Children also demonstrate an increased belief that prayer will work between the ages of 4 and 8 (Woolley and Phelps 2001). Children between the ages of 6 and 11 from varied religious backgrounds in the United States indicate prayer would be more effective than wishing or magic to prevent an unwanted, negative outcome from occurring (Payir et al 2022). However, between the ages of 3 and 11, children from varied religious backgrounds in the United States are also more likely to claim that events that are probable are more likely to occur through prayer than events that are physically impossible (Lane 2020).…”
Section: Asking God For Help: Children's Views On What To Pray For Whenmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Religions 2023, 14, 1164 2 of 18 Furthermore, studies documented the conditions under which God would likely answer a prayer, based on the content of the prayer (Woolley and Phelps 2001), whether the request is for something that is improbable or impossible (Lane 2020;Payir et al 2022), or even whether prayers are silent or spoken out loud (Lane et al 2016). However, little is known about what kinds of things children believe God actually does to fulfill or answer a prayer.…”
Section: Asking God For Help: Children's Views On What To Pray For Whenmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Similarly, studies have examined the kinds of ideas children propose when asked to consider what could have been done to prevent or change the outcome of an event (i.e., counterfactual alternatives). Although both religious and nonreligious children appeal to natural causes as the primary mechanism considered, religious children are also significantly more likely to view supernatural causes as an equally plausible alternative (Payir et al 2022). With age, children increasingly engage in probabilistic reasoning to make inferences about the likelihood of a causal mechanism resulting in a desired action; nevertheless, religious children view religious mechanisms such as praying as more probable than similar nonreligious acts (e.g., wishing; Lane 2020).…”
Section: Religious Agentsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, even when children are asked to evaluate the plausibility of preventive measures presented to them by an adult rather than generated by their own reflection, they typically endorse the efficacy of naturalistic interventions. Nonetheless, religious children, unlike their secular peers, occasionally endorse the efficacy of divine intervention (Payir et al 2022). In sum, whether children are asked to assess the real-world plausibility of a sequence of events, or to imagine how it might have turned out differently, their primary recourse is to naturalistic thinking.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%