2022
DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2021.10.005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

What can we learn from inactivation studies? Lessons from auditory cortex

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

1
28
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(29 citation statements)
references
References 110 publications
1
28
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The behavioral necessity of a brain area is typically assessed using loss-of-function perturbations. Across sensory modalities, such experiments suggest that primary sensory cortices are essential for some tasks but not others ( Lashley, 1931a , b ; Schneider, 1969 ; Hutson and Masterton, 1986 ; Newsome and Paré, 1988 ; Prusky and Douglas, 2004 ; Glickfeld et al, 2013 ; Shih et al, 2013 ; Poort et al, 2015 ; Goard et al, 2016 ; Resulaj et al, 2018 ; Stüttgen and Schwarz, 2018 ; Slonina et al, 2022 ). Typical loss-of-function experiments inactivate spatially extensive tissue volumes and rarely allow for behavioral recovery, complicating interpretation and leaving open the question of whether small cortical volumes act as permanent perceptual bottlenecks.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The behavioral necessity of a brain area is typically assessed using loss-of-function perturbations. Across sensory modalities, such experiments suggest that primary sensory cortices are essential for some tasks but not others ( Lashley, 1931a , b ; Schneider, 1969 ; Hutson and Masterton, 1986 ; Newsome and Paré, 1988 ; Prusky and Douglas, 2004 ; Glickfeld et al, 2013 ; Shih et al, 2013 ; Poort et al, 2015 ; Goard et al, 2016 ; Resulaj et al, 2018 ; Stüttgen and Schwarz, 2018 ; Slonina et al, 2022 ). Typical loss-of-function experiments inactivate spatially extensive tissue volumes and rarely allow for behavioral recovery, complicating interpretation and leaving open the question of whether small cortical volumes act as permanent perceptual bottlenecks.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Object location discrimination requires cortex, whereas detection does not. In the auditory cortex, many discrimination behaviors require cortex (Lomber and Malhotra, 2008; Slonina et al, 2022). At the same time, simple auditory features such as frequency can be discriminated even without cortex (Ohl et al, 1999).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The behavioral necessity of a brain area is typically assessed by performing loss-of-function perturbations. Across sensory modalities, such experiments suggest that primary sensory cortices are essential for some tasks but not others (Lashley, 1931a, b; Schneider, 1969; Hutson and Masterton, 1986; Newsome and Pare, 1988; Prusky and Douglas, 2004; Glickfeld et al, 2013; Shih et al, 2013; Poort et al, 2015; Goard et al, 2016; Resulaj et al, 2018; Stuttgen and Schwarz, 2018; Slonina et al, 2022). Traditional loss-of-function perturbations, however, have poor spatial resolution: both transient inactivation (O’Connor et al, 2010; Poort et al, 2015; Goard et al, 2016; Li et al, 2019) and permanent lesions (Lashley, 1931a; Hutson and Masterton, 1986; Resulaj et al, 2018) usually have a radius of effect exceeding 1 mm, thereby impacting hundreds of thousands of neurons.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Extensive research has shown how sound localization cues are extracted in the midbrain (Tollin and Yin, 2002;Yin and Chan, 1990) and transmitted across the brain to areas, including auditory cortex (Keating et al, 2015;Stecker et al, 2005), parietal and prefrontal cortex (van der Heijden et al, 2019). While midbrain neurons show tuning to specific localization cues, auditory cortex contains cue-invariant representations of sound location (Wood et al, 2019) and is essential for sound localization in primates and carnivores (Slonina et al, 2022).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%