2019
DOI: 10.1057/s41290-019-00077-8
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

What can cognitive neuroscience do for cultural sociology?

Abstract: Can cognitive neuroscience contribute to cultural sociology? We argue that it can, but to profit from such contributions requires developing coherent positions at the level of ontology and coherent epistemological views concerning interfield relations in science. In this paper, we carve out a coherent position that makes sense for cultural sociology based on Sperber's "infraindividualist" and Clark's "extended cognition" arguments. More substantively, we take on three canonical topics in cultural sociology: la… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

0
36
0
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 32 publications
(41 citation statements)
references
References 152 publications
0
36
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The goal, then, is to assign each word a single vector such that the "gradedness in distributional representations correlates with gradedness in semantic phenomena" (Boleda 2020:228) . 17 This procedure of inferring a word's meaning by summarizing its (linguistic) context aligns with relational theories of culture-pioneered by John Mohr (Kirchner and Mohr 2010;Mohr 1998Mohr , 2000 Hinton 1986;Ignatow 2007Ignatow , 2016Landauer and Dumais 1997;Lizardo et al 2019;Osgood 1952;Strauss and Quinn 1997;Turner 2011;Zaromb et al 2006) .…”
Section: Language Modeling and Relational Theories Of Meaningmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…The goal, then, is to assign each word a single vector such that the "gradedness in distributional representations correlates with gradedness in semantic phenomena" (Boleda 2020:228) . 17 This procedure of inferring a word's meaning by summarizing its (linguistic) context aligns with relational theories of culture-pioneered by John Mohr (Kirchner and Mohr 2010;Mohr 1998Mohr , 2000 Hinton 1986;Ignatow 2007Ignatow , 2016Landauer and Dumais 1997;Lizardo et al 2019;Osgood 1952;Strauss and Quinn 1997;Turner 2011;Zaromb et al 2006) .…”
Section: Language Modeling and Relational Theories Of Meaningmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…In the past two decades, literature on how the cognitive sciences can aid cultural analyses has grown exponentially (e.g. Cerulo 2002Cerulo , 2010DiMaggio 1997;Kaidesoja et al 2019;Lizardo et al 2020;Zerubavel 1997). This "cognitive turn" has contributed to improved conceptualizations of different types of culture (Lizardo 2017;Patterson 2014), consideration of the body's role in cultural knowledge (Ignatow 2007), attention to automatic cognitions at the subconscious level (Vaisey 2009), and new discussions about methods for studying culture (Cerulo 2014;Jerolmack and Khan 2014;Martin 2010;Pugh 2013;Summers-Effler et al 2015;Swidler 1986Swidler , 2001Vaisey 2009Vaisey , 2014.…”
Section: The Cognitive Turn In Cultural Analysesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A shared premise of the so-called "cognitive culturalists" (Pugh 2013) is that understanding cultural knowledge and the discourses and actions it leads to requires understanding the cognitive bases of cultural knowledge. Making claims about culture's coherence, consistency, composition, location, role in motivation, or other ways it "works" inevitably involves assumptions about human cognition, whether stated explicitly or assumed implicitly (Bloch 2012;DiMaggio 1997;Foster 2018;Kaidesoja et al 2019;Lizardo et al 2020;Smith et al 2020;Strauss and Quinn 1997). Thus, "we are better off if we make such models explicit than if we smuggle then in through the back door" (DiMaggio 1997, p. 282).…”
Section: The Cognitive Turn In Cultural Analysesmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations