2010
DOI: 10.1037/a0019996
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Visuospatial attention is guided by both the symbolic value and the spatial proximity of selected arrows.

Abstract: There is considerable evidence that overlearned symbols, especially arrows, can orient attention to peripheral locations. In 2003, Pratt and Hommel showed that when 1 arrow is selected from a set of arrows, based on an attentional control setting for a specific target color, the selected arrow determines the orientation of attention. Recently, Leblanc and Jolicoeur (2010) reexamined this finding, and concluded that spatial proximity of the arrow to the target, not the symbolic value of the arrow, determines th… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

3
10
0

Year Published

2011
2011
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
3
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This validity effect reflects shifts of attention following the arrow cue, which result in facilitated target processing after valid cues and hampered target processing after invalid cues because of the necessity to reorient attention to the target location. This result replicates the finding that endogenous cues with a strong spatial meaning, like arrows, impact on attention akin to exogenous cues (Hommel et al, 2001; Pratt et al, 2010; Tipples, 2002). …”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 86%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…This validity effect reflects shifts of attention following the arrow cue, which result in facilitated target processing after valid cues and hampered target processing after invalid cues because of the necessity to reorient attention to the target location. This result replicates the finding that endogenous cues with a strong spatial meaning, like arrows, impact on attention akin to exogenous cues (Hommel et al, 2001; Pratt et al, 2010; Tipples, 2002). …”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 86%
“…We were able to replicate findings that visible, centrally presented arrows trigger automatic shifts of attention (Friesen et al, 2004; Gibson & Bryant, 2005; Hommel et al, 2001; Pratt et al, 2010; Tipples, 2002). Most importantly, masked arrow cues also triggered shifts of attention, yet only when overall cue validity was 80%, whereas masked cues remained ineffective when overall cue validity was 50%.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 63%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Furthermore, symbolic cuing is disrupted by dual-task interference in the precue setting (Du & Abrams, 2010). Yet, precues, particularly arrow cues, have also been shown to effectively bias attention automatically, even when they are uninformative (e.g., Eimer, 1997; Pratt, Radulescu, Guo, & Hommel, 2010). Although one distinction of the retrocue literature from the precue literature is that the nature of the cue is important and neither purely symbolic cues nor purely exogenous cues elicit the RCE (Berryhill et al, 2011), it seems reasonable that arrow retro-cues could automatically trigger processes giving rise to the RCE, because arrow cues reliably elicit the RCE (even though there are known differences between external and internal attention; Chun, Golomb, & Turk-Browne, 2011; Makovski & Jiang, 2007; Tanoue, Jones, Peterson, & Berryhill, 2013).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Spatial cuing, Symbolic attention control . Voluntary attention controlSpatial symbols are often used to elicit voluntary shifts of attention in the laboratory, though these symbols may also elicit involuntary (or automated) shifts of attention (Friesen & Kingstone, 1998;Friesen, Ristic, & Kingstone, 2004;Gibson & Bryant, 2005;Ho & Spence, 2006;Hommel, Pratt, Colzato, & Godijn, 2001;Jonides, 1981;Leblanc & Jolicoeur, 2010;Pratt, Radulescu, Guo, & Hommel, 2010;Ristic, Friesen, & Kingstone, 2002;Ristic & Kingstone, 2006Tipples, 2002Tipples, , 2008. For the past 30 years, the primary tool used to study attentional shifts in response to such symbolic cues has been the spatial cuing paradigm (Posner, 1980;Posner, Snyder, & Davidson, 1980).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%