2015
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0117901
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Visuomotor Adaptation: How Forgetting Keeps Us Conservative

Abstract: Even when provided with feedback after every movement, adaptation levels off before biases are completely removed. Incomplete adaptation has recently been attributed to forgetting: the adaptation is already partially forgotten by the time the next movement is made. Here we test whether this idea is correct. If so, the final level of adaptation is determined by a balance between learning and forgetting. Because we learn from perceived errors, scaling these errors by a magnification factor has the same effect as… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

5
31
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 40 publications
(36 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
5
31
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Since the motor system seems to be attempting to eliminate these asymptotic errors, stable but incomplete asymptotic adaptation can only be achieved as an equilibrium between the new learning on each trial (usually 5–20% of the remaining error [ 2 , 19 , 29 ]) and the same amount of decay on each trial. This idea is further supported by a recent study from van der Kooij et al [ 30 ], who manipulated the balance between new learning and decay by magnifying or shrinking endpoint feedback during learning, which should affect the learning on each trial but not the decay. They demonstrated that when the error signal was magnified, thereby increasing the drive for error-dependent learning, asymptotic adaptation increased compared to veridical error display because the stronger learning required less true error to balance the decay.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 81%
“…Since the motor system seems to be attempting to eliminate these asymptotic errors, stable but incomplete asymptotic adaptation can only be achieved as an equilibrium between the new learning on each trial (usually 5–20% of the remaining error [ 2 , 19 , 29 ]) and the same amount of decay on each trial. This idea is further supported by a recent study from van der Kooij et al [ 30 ], who manipulated the balance between new learning and decay by magnifying or shrinking endpoint feedback during learning, which should affect the learning on each trial but not the decay. They demonstrated that when the error signal was magnified, thereby increasing the drive for error-dependent learning, asymptotic adaptation increased compared to veridical error display because the stronger learning required less true error to balance the decay.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 81%
“…Computationally, our model can be seen as a generalization of forgetting models of incomplete adaptation [ 24 ]: Namely, by replacing the term in Eq (4) by a constant term α , we find that the level of adaptation is set by two terms among which the target pitch μ * has the role of driving a forgetting term. Our nonlinear model is more general than simple forgetting-retention models in the sense that it correctly explains zero adaptation for highly magnified errors ( Fig 2a ) and that adaptation time constants are not fixed but depend on posterior evidence.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Why would one remember an action or aiming direction that was ultimately unsuccessful (i.e., led to a target miss), as is often the case given the bias toward baseline often exhibited at asymptote (Kitago et al 2013;van der Kooij et al 2015;Vaswani et al 2015)? It is plausible that a memory for action could be formed because of strong positive reward prediction errors experienced by subjects during the initial course of adaptation.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%