1963
DOI: 10.1097/00006324-196309000-00006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Visual Sensory Units and the Minimum Angle of Resolution

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
15
0

Year Published

1969
1969
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
2
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It is a reasonable assumption that the kind of acuity task that most closely parallels size-difference discrimination is the recognition of the orientation of the gap in a Landolt C. Using data from Shlaer (1937), the minimum angle of resolution (the reciprocal of visual acuity) for the gap in a Landolt C for central vision at a luminance equivalent to that used in the present experiments is found to be approximately 0.45 min, which corresponds closely to the mean of 0.48 min obtained in the present experiment from all Ss for all stimulus sizes (SD =0.036 min). For peripheral vision the data in Fig.4 that are taken from Westheimer (1967) show close agreement with the regression line from the present experiments; it should be noted that Westheimer's data were obtained from eccentricities less than IO deg; the acuity task in this case was again gap detection in a Landolt C. Weymouth (1958) has pointed out that functions that are linear with eccentricity are typically found for all types of acuity tests; he shows that intercept values go from 0.43 min for vernier acuity to 4.6 min for one S for the Landolt C (figures are again minimum angle of resolution). Slope values were found to vary from about 0.11 for motion acuity to 1.80 for the Landolt C. The intercept and slope values from the present experiments were 0.6 and 0.88, respectively, which fall within the error range of Weymouth's peripheral visual acuity data for 20 Ss on a Landolt C task.…”
Section: Size Discrimination In Peripheral Visionsupporting
confidence: 85%
“…It is a reasonable assumption that the kind of acuity task that most closely parallels size-difference discrimination is the recognition of the orientation of the gap in a Landolt C. Using data from Shlaer (1937), the minimum angle of resolution (the reciprocal of visual acuity) for the gap in a Landolt C for central vision at a luminance equivalent to that used in the present experiments is found to be approximately 0.45 min, which corresponds closely to the mean of 0.48 min obtained in the present experiment from all Ss for all stimulus sizes (SD =0.036 min). For peripheral vision the data in Fig.4 that are taken from Westheimer (1967) show close agreement with the regression line from the present experiments; it should be noted that Westheimer's data were obtained from eccentricities less than IO deg; the acuity task in this case was again gap detection in a Landolt C. Weymouth (1958) has pointed out that functions that are linear with eccentricity are typically found for all types of acuity tests; he shows that intercept values go from 0.43 min for vernier acuity to 4.6 min for one S for the Landolt C (figures are again minimum angle of resolution). Slope values were found to vary from about 0.11 for motion acuity to 1.80 for the Landolt C. The intercept and slope values from the present experiments were 0.6 and 0.88, respectively, which fall within the error range of Weymouth's peripheral visual acuity data for 20 Ss on a Landolt C task.…”
Section: Size Discrimination In Peripheral Visionsupporting
confidence: 85%
“…When measured in terms of a minimum angle of resolution (MAR), rather than its reciprocal, acuity, a linear model matches both anatomical data (e.g. receptor density), as well as performance results on many low-level vision tasks [Weymouth 1958;Strasburger et al 2011]. Acuity models form the basis for the well-known theory of cortical magnification or M-scaling, which posits that enlarging a visual stimulus minimizes performance variation as it becomes increasingly peripheral.…”
Section: Human Visual Acuity In the Peripherymentioning
confidence: 91%
“…We expect that, if third-order motion is perceived, it would be perceived at low temporal frequencies and in central vision, because Lu and Sperling (l995b) found that the third order (salience motion) mechanism has a much lower cutoff frequency (at 3 Hz) than does the motion energy system and it has coarser spatial resolution. Indeed, previous stimuli that were constructed to evade the first-order motion system (and that probably stimulated a combination of second-and third-order motion) have been found to require lower temporal and spatial frequencies than do first-order stimuli (Anstis, 1974;Millodot, Johnson, Lamont, & Leibowitz, 1975;Weymouth, 1958). So, in appropriately chosen viewing conditions (fovea, low temporal frequencies), it might be possible for the third-order system to dominate the motion extraction process.…”
Section: Apparatusmentioning
confidence: 99%