2010
DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1009438107
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Visual motion aftereffect from understanding motion language

Abstract: Do people spontaneously form visual mental images when understanding language, and if so, how truly visual are these representations? We test whether processing linguistic descriptions of motion produces sufficiently vivid mental images to cause direction-selective motion adaptation in the visual system (i.e., cause a motion aftereffect illusion). We tested for motion aftereffects (MAEs) following explicit motion imagery, and after processing literal or metaphorical motion language (without instructions to ima… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
46
1

Year Published

2012
2012
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 54 publications
(51 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
3
46
1
Order By: Relevance
“…For instance, reading a sound-related word like "thunder" activates the auditory association cortex, and aphasic patients with atrophy of the auditory association cortex have impaired processing of such words (Bonner & Grossman, 2012; see also Boulenger et al, 2008;Goldberg, Perfetti, & Schneider, 2006;Hauk, Johnsrude, & PulvermLller, 2004). Across a range of paradigms, behavioural experimentation has also revealed complex interactions between the representations required for processing the environment and those simulated for conceptual processing (e.g., , 2012, 2014aDils & Boroditsky, 2010;Zwaan & Taylor, 2006). For example, people are faster to make size judgements about manipulable objects when they are holding a ball between the hands, due to the importance of haptic perception in experiencing the size of apples and grapes (Connell, Lynott, & Dreyer, 2012).…”
Section: Concepts As Simulated and Linguistic Informationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, reading a sound-related word like "thunder" activates the auditory association cortex, and aphasic patients with atrophy of the auditory association cortex have impaired processing of such words (Bonner & Grossman, 2012; see also Boulenger et al, 2008;Goldberg, Perfetti, & Schneider, 2006;Hauk, Johnsrude, & PulvermLller, 2004). Across a range of paradigms, behavioural experimentation has also revealed complex interactions between the representations required for processing the environment and those simulated for conceptual processing (e.g., , 2012, 2014aDils & Boroditsky, 2010;Zwaan & Taylor, 2006). For example, people are faster to make size judgements about manipulable objects when they are holding a ball between the hands, due to the importance of haptic perception in experiencing the size of apples and grapes (Connell, Lynott, & Dreyer, 2012).…”
Section: Concepts As Simulated and Linguistic Informationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There is now evidence that many cognitive processes, such as moral reasoning (26), language comprehension (27), autobiographical memory, imagining the future (28), dreams (29), and expectations about upcoming tasks (30), all involve sensory representations. What remains unclear, however, is the precise representational format of these visual mental representations.…”
Section: Next Chaptermentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In particular, we asked which way, after seeing rightward or leftward motion for 30 s, they would expect a stationary pattern to appear to move (Dils & Boroditsky, 2010).…”
Section: Real-and Implied-motion-adaptation Experimentsmentioning
confidence: 99%