2006
DOI: 10.1007/s00221-006-0767-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Visual information throughout a reach determines endpoint precision

Abstract: People make rapid, goal-directed movements to interact with their environment. Because these movements have consequences, it is important to be able to control them with a high level of precision and accuracy. Our hypothesis is that vision guides rapid hand movements, thereby enhancing their accuracy and precision. To test this idea, we asked observers to point to a briefly presented target (110 ms). We measured the impact of visual information on endpoint precision by using a shutter to close off view of the … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
21
0

Year Published

2008
2008
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 35 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
2
21
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Given that aiming tasks have been shown to rely heavily on visual information (Todor and Kyprie 1980), results reported by Flowers (1975) suggest an advantage for the preferred right arm in the utilization of visual feedback. In support of this conclusion, arm differences have also been demonstrated in the kinematics of visually guided aiming, especially in the case of deceleration duration, a period of time during which on-line corrections in movement trajectory based on visual feedback may occur (Carlton 1981;Elliott et al 1999a;Ma-Wyatt and McKee 2007). In this case, deceleration duration has been found to be shorter for the preferred arm (Boulinguez et al 2001;Elliott et al 1995;Mieschke et al 2001;Roy et al 1994;Todor and Cisneros 1985), suggesting a preferred-arm specialization for the more effective processing of visual feedback.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 71%
“…Given that aiming tasks have been shown to rely heavily on visual information (Todor and Kyprie 1980), results reported by Flowers (1975) suggest an advantage for the preferred right arm in the utilization of visual feedback. In support of this conclusion, arm differences have also been demonstrated in the kinematics of visually guided aiming, especially in the case of deceleration duration, a period of time during which on-line corrections in movement trajectory based on visual feedback may occur (Carlton 1981;Elliott et al 1999a;Ma-Wyatt and McKee 2007). In this case, deceleration duration has been found to be shorter for the preferred arm (Boulinguez et al 2001;Elliott et al 1995;Mieschke et al 2001;Roy et al 1994;Todor and Cisneros 1985), suggesting a preferred-arm specialization for the more effective processing of visual feedback.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 71%
“…For instance, with small movement targets (Fitts, 1954), it is possible to plan a slower but more accurate movement. However, visual feedback can also be used to control and change the movement trajectory during movement execution (Ma-Wyatt & McKee, 2007). Thus, it is possible that low-salience targets give rise to the planning of slower movements (compared to high-salience targets), and/or high-salience targets permit improved online control of the movement and thus faster movement execution.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Onset latencies were examined because these reflect processing prior to the overt pointing movement (Glover, 2004; see also Song & Nakayama, 2006, though we also analyzed the movement durations. Changes in movement time can stem from the (preparatory) planning stage of movement (Fitts, 1954;Glover, 2004), from processes that control and modulate the movement during its execution (Ma-Wyatt & McKee, 2007), and/or from modulations of movement trajectories (Song & Nakayama, 2006).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is well established that our actions are strongly guided by visual perception: Activities such as reaching, grasping, and pointing to objects are performed more accurately and faster when they occur within our visual field and, in particular, when presented within the current focus of our visual attention (e.g., Adam, Buetti, & Kerzel, 2012b;Castiello, 1999;Ma-Wyatt & McKee, 2007). Effects of action on visual perception, in turn, are more subtle and have only recently been studied (e.g., Bekkering & Neggers, 2002;Fagioli, Hommel, & Schubotz, 2007;Vishton et al, 2007;Wohlschläger, 2000).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%