2011
DOI: 10.1002/da.20791
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Vigilant and avoidant attention biases as predictors of response to cognitive behavioral therapy for social phobia

Abstract: Background Attention bias for socially threatening information, an empirically supported phenomenon, figures prominently in models of social phobia. However, all published studies examining this topic to date have relied on group means to describe attention bias patterns; research has yet to examine potential subgroups of attention bias among individuals with social phobia (e.g., vigilant or avoidant). Furthermore, almost no research has examined how attention biases in either direction may predict change in s… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

9
60
1

Year Published

2012
2012
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 62 publications
(70 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
9
60
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Based on previous findings of elevated responding in anxious relative to control children to both CSs during acquisition and/or extinction (e.g., Craske, Kircanski, et al, 2008;Craske, Waters, et al, 2008;Lau et al, 2008;Waters, Henry, et al, 2009), we hypothesised that these effects would be most pronounced in anxious children with a threat avoidant versus a threat vigilant attention bias. Specifically, we predicted that in comparison to threat vigilant anxious children, those who avoided threat would (a) be more reactive to an aversive US on CSþ trials as well as CSÀ trials, as indexed by skin conductance response magnitude (SCRs) (e.g., Craske, Kircanski, et al, 2008;Craske, Waters, et al, 2008;Waters, Henry, et al, 2009), (b) display larger SCRs to both CSs during extinction trials (e.g., Craske, Kircanski, et al, 2008;Craske, Waters, et al, 2008), (c) rate the CSþ and CSÀ as more unpleasant and arousing after acquisition and extinction (e.g., Lau et al, 2008), and (d) report higher subjective anxiety ratings post-extinction, consistent with previous findings of poorer outcomes following exposure-based CBT in threat avoidant compared to threat vigilant anxious adults and children (e.g., Niles et al, 2013;Price et al, 2011;Waters et al, 2012).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 75%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Based on previous findings of elevated responding in anxious relative to control children to both CSs during acquisition and/or extinction (e.g., Craske, Kircanski, et al, 2008;Craske, Waters, et al, 2008;Lau et al, 2008;Waters, Henry, et al, 2009), we hypothesised that these effects would be most pronounced in anxious children with a threat avoidant versus a threat vigilant attention bias. Specifically, we predicted that in comparison to threat vigilant anxious children, those who avoided threat would (a) be more reactive to an aversive US on CSþ trials as well as CSÀ trials, as indexed by skin conductance response magnitude (SCRs) (e.g., Craske, Kircanski, et al, 2008;Craske, Waters, et al, 2008;Waters, Henry, et al, 2009), (b) display larger SCRs to both CSs during extinction trials (e.g., Craske, Kircanski, et al, 2008;Craske, Waters, et al, 2008), (c) rate the CSþ and CSÀ as more unpleasant and arousing after acquisition and extinction (e.g., Lau et al, 2008), and (d) report higher subjective anxiety ratings post-extinction, consistent with previous findings of poorer outcomes following exposure-based CBT in threat avoidant compared to threat vigilant anxious adults and children (e.g., Niles et al, 2013;Price et al, 2011;Waters et al, 2012).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 75%
“…The clinical translation of these effects, whether due to underlying associative, non-associative or combined mechanisms, may be that threat avoidant anxious children are slower than threat vigilant anxious children to respond to the same 'dose' of exposure therapy. In accord, several studies have found poorer outcomes following exposure-based CBT in anxious adults and children who are threat avoidant compared to threat vigilant (e.g., Niles, Mesri, Burklund, Lieberman, & Craske, 2013;Price et al, 2011;Waters et al, 2012).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 81%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Other studies, which have been designed to assess relationships between attentional bias and CBT outcome in larger samples of anxious children, have provided mixed results, suggesting that pre-treatment attention bias towards threat may predict poorer outcome (e.g., Legerstee et al 2010;Manassis et al 2013) or better outcome (Waters et al 2012; see also Price et al 2011, for similar findings in anxious adults). Such mixed results may be due to variation in experimental methods for assessing attentional bias (e.g., severity of threat stimuli; different tasks, see Waters et al 2012, for further discussion).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…In addition to examining the correlations between symptom change and pre-treatment threat bias scores, the latter was also dichotomised to reflect 'threat bias direction' in order to allow direct comparison with previous research (Price et al, 2011;Waters et al, 2012), whereby children were classified as threat vigilant (pre-treatment threat bias score > zero; n = 15) or avoidant (score < zero; n = 22). The effect of pre-treatment happy face bias scores was examined in the same way.…”
Section: A C C E P T E D Accepted Manuscriptmentioning
confidence: 99%