2001
DOI: 10.1111/1467-9280.00307
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Viewpoint Dependence in Visual and Haptic Object Recognition

Abstract: Abstract-On the whole, people recognize objects best when they see the objects from a familiar view and worse when they see the objects from views that were previously occluded from sight. Unexpectedly, we found haptic object recognition to be viewpoint-specific as well, even though hand movements were unrestricted. This viewpoint dependence was due to the hands preferring the back "view" of the objects. Furthermore, when the sensory modalities (visual vs. haptic) differed between learning an object and reco… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

18
188
7
2

Year Published

2003
2003
2012
2012

Publication Types

Select...
4
4

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 206 publications
(215 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
18
188
7
2
Order By: Relevance
“…We normally look at what our hands are doing, and attentional resources could become dominated by the visual channel in these circumstances. In turn, beneficial effects of visual experience on haptic identification of particularly (familiar) 3-D stimuli (Shimizu et al 1993) may result from similar (Newell et al 2001;James et al 2002) or common representations of haptic and visual form (Reales and Ballesteros 1999) or from a unidirectional enhancement, ie visual experience enhances haptic processing (cf Sathian et al 1997;Amedi et al 2001;Pascual-Leone and Hamilton 2001). In addition, for the inferior performance of the early-blind in 2-D identification tasks (Heller 1989b, experiment 2;Shimizu et al 1993), several theoretical explanations have been put forward, such as problems with making decisions (Heller 1989b), and/or dependence on egocentric coding (Millar 1981;Gaunet et al 1997).…”
Section: Object Processingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We normally look at what our hands are doing, and attentional resources could become dominated by the visual channel in these circumstances. In turn, beneficial effects of visual experience on haptic identification of particularly (familiar) 3-D stimuli (Shimizu et al 1993) may result from similar (Newell et al 2001;James et al 2002) or common representations of haptic and visual form (Reales and Ballesteros 1999) or from a unidirectional enhancement, ie visual experience enhances haptic processing (cf Sathian et al 1997;Amedi et al 2001;Pascual-Leone and Hamilton 2001). In addition, for the inferior performance of the early-blind in 2-D identification tasks (Heller 1989b, experiment 2;Shimizu et al 1993), several theoretical explanations have been put forward, such as problems with making decisions (Heller 1989b), and/or dependence on egocentric coding (Millar 1981;Gaunet et al 1997).…”
Section: Object Processingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…(Replotted from Newell et al 2001. ) In an effort to examine what shape information is encoded in vision and touch, respectively, in a recent recognition memory study we used unfamiliar Lego™ objects as stimuli presented in a fixed position (Newell et al, 2001) (c.f. Figure 2).…”
Section: Cross-modal Object Recognitionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For the encoding of shape information, haptics is generally slower than vision (e.g., Jones, 1981;Newell et al, 2001;Woods, O'Modhrain, & Newell, 2004) and consequently performance was worse for haptic than for visual learning. In our first Experiment (1a), the learning times for both vision and touch were limited to 30 s each.…”
Section: Active Visual and Haptic Explorationmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Thus, Newell et al (2002) reported that the backs of objects were the preferred vantage points for touch. Viewpoint is closely linked to linear perspective, since perspective will vary with vantage point (Heller et al 2002.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%