2016
DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00017
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Viewing Instructions Accompanying Action Observation Modulate Corticospinal Excitability

Abstract: Action observation interventions may have the potential to contribute to improved motor function in motor (re)learning settings by promoting functional activity and plasticity in the motor regions of the brain. Optimal methods for delivering such interventions, however, have yet to be established. This experiment investigated the effect on corticospinal excitability of manipulating the viewing instructions provided to participants (N = 21) prior to action observation. Specifically, motor evoked potential respo… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
32
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 35 publications
(34 citation statements)
references
References 56 publications
2
32
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Here as well, no superiority effect of AO+MI on muscle function was found when compared to the AO+Attention, while both AO+Attention and AO+MI outperformed the control condition in terms of total force. Simultaneous MI and AO were found to significantly enhance corticomotor excitability in comparison to pure AO (Wright et al, 2016;Cengiz et al, 2018). Being instructed to observe passively, or with the intent to imitate the observed movement, or while simultaneously and actively imagining self-performance of the movement during observation facilitated corticospinal excitability to a greater extent than observation of a static hand.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 87%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Here as well, no superiority effect of AO+MI on muscle function was found when compared to the AO+Attention, while both AO+Attention and AO+MI outperformed the control condition in terms of total force. Simultaneous MI and AO were found to significantly enhance corticomotor excitability in comparison to pure AO (Wright et al, 2016;Cengiz et al, 2018). Being instructed to observe passively, or with the intent to imitate the observed movement, or while simultaneously and actively imagining self-performance of the movement during observation facilitated corticospinal excitability to a greater extent than observation of a static hand.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…Being instructed to observe passively, or with the intent to imitate the observed movement, or while simultaneously and actively imagining self-performance of the movement during observation facilitated corticospinal excitability to a greater extent than observation of a static hand. In addition, corticospinal excitability was facilitated to a greater extent during combined observation and imagery as compared to passive observation (Wright et al, 2016).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…First, corticospinal excitability, measured through the amplitudes of motor evoked potentials, during both AO and MI of hand gestures is reliably higher than control conditions (e.g., Clark et al, 2004; Williams et al, 2012; see Naish et al, 2014; Grosprêtre et al, 2016 for reviews). Second, AO+MI produces significantly greater facilitation of corticospinal excitability compared to AO (Ohno et al, 2011; Wright et al, 2014, 2016) and, in some cases, MI as well (Sakamoto et al, 2009; Tsukazaki et al, 2012; Mouthon et al, 2015). These effects have been demonstrated across a variety of tasks, including simple and sequential finger movements (Wright et al, 2014, 2016), gross and fine motor tasks (Sakamoto et al, 2009; Ohno et al, 2011) and coordination tasks (Tsukazaki et al, 2012; Mouthon et al, 2015).…”
Section: The Effects Of Motor Imagery During Action Observation: Empimentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Several studies have filmed the AO component from a first-person visual perspective (e.g., Villiger et al, 2013; Wright et al, 2014, 2016), while other studies have filmed the action from a third-person visual perspective (e.g., Eaves et al, 2014, 2016; Mouthon et al, 2015; Taube et al, 2015). In some cases, participants are instructed to explicitly image from a first person perspective, while in other cases they are only told to imagine themselves performing the observed movement, which may result in participants adopting either a first- or third-person imagery perspective, depending on their imagery perspective preference.…”
Section: Future Research Opportunitiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In order to determine the sample size, a statistical power analysis was conducted using the G � Power software package. Since we were mainly interested in the effects of an observer's visual experience (factor A) or intention to imitate (factor B) on corticospinal excitability, the effect size for the analysis of factor A was set at f = 0.595 based on data from a previous between-groups TMS study [37], and that for factor B was set at f = 0.850 based on data from similar published TMS research [38], with an alpha of 0.05 and a power of 0.80. The minimum sample size necessary for this experiment was thus estimated to be n = 25.…”
Section: Participantsmentioning
confidence: 99%