2009
DOI: 10.1080/09541440802453006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Vierordt'sThe Experimental Study of the Time Sense(1868) and its legacy

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

25
184
2
3

Year Published

2011
2011
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 226 publications
(214 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
(25 reference statements)
25
184
2
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Studies using simple sensorimotor tasks suggest that human participants are adept at learning the distribution of sets of stimuli and integrating this acquired knowledge with sensory evidence when making decisions and planning actions (4,(10)(11)(12)(13). For example, a classic finding in temporal reproduction experiments is that judgments converge toward a central valueparticipants consistently overestimate shorter durations and underestimate longer ones (14,15). The center of convergence at which there is no constant reproduction error (termed the "indifference point") is not fixed but rather, shifts depending on the set of durations presented (16,17).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Studies using simple sensorimotor tasks suggest that human participants are adept at learning the distribution of sets of stimuli and integrating this acquired knowledge with sensory evidence when making decisions and planning actions (4,(10)(11)(12)(13). For example, a classic finding in temporal reproduction experiments is that judgments converge toward a central valueparticipants consistently overestimate shorter durations and underestimate longer ones (14,15). The center of convergence at which there is no constant reproduction error (termed the "indifference point") is not fixed but rather, shifts depending on the set of durations presented (16,17).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A long history of timing research has shown that, similar to other physical properties, the subjective experience of the same physical duration can greatly vary depending on the context in which it is embedded (for reviews, see Bausenhart, Bratzke, & Ulrich, 2016;Shi, Church, & Meck, 2013). A famous example of such context effects is the Vierordt effect (Lejeune & Wearden, 2009;Vierordt, 1868): Within a certain range of presented intervals, relatively long intervals are underestimated and relatively short intervals are overestimated. Recent explanations for such context effects differ in their exact mechanism, but they all share the idea that not only the current trial information but also the temporal context is taken into account when participants provide duration estimates or comparison judgments (Bausenhart et al, 2016;Dyjas, Bausenhart, & Ulrich, 2012;Shi et al, 2013).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, these results point at a migration of reproduced durations (a.k.a. memory mixing), possibly because the three durations were randomly assigned to each trial rather than being blocked (i.e., Vierordt's law; Gu & Meck, 2011;Lejeune & Wearden, 2009). Additionally, an unexpected result was found in Experiment 1, where reproductions in trials in which the static stimulus was encoded or decoded were longer than the target duration.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 78%