1995
DOI: 10.1515/ling.1995.33.3.403
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Verbal suppletion: an analysis of Italian, French, and Spanish to go

Abstract: Analyses of suppletive verb conjugations have focused on aspect or tense divisions, type and token frequency, and lexicalization of individual forms at an earlier stage of the language. In this paper, I attempt to show that person-number suppletive replacements follow language-specific alternation patterns, or templates, which mark the positions of stem alternations often shared by several verbs. These paradigmatic frameworks are stored in the lexicon with their verb forms and guide verbal restructuring proces… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
12
0
1

Year Published

1997
1997
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 46 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
1
12
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…13 More arguments for the paradigm are given in Börjars et al (1997). 14 The same observation is made in Aski (1995).…”
Section: Address Of the Authormentioning
confidence: 89%
“…13 More arguments for the paradigm are given in Börjars et al (1997). 14 The same observation is made in Aski (1995).…”
Section: Address Of the Authormentioning
confidence: 89%
“…Nor is there any sense in which the plurals of mik and ˈmari are likely to have been morphologically or phonologically 'deficient' , for example by being excessively reduced phonologically or somehow already irregular or archaic (cf. Rudes 1980, Aski 1995 and also the discussion in Börjars and Vincent 2011, p. 144). The plural of ˈmari, as we have seen, actually does exist and it is simply mar, while that of mik should be *mits (in fact attested by Weigand, as a noun).…”
Section: The Genesis Of the Megleno-romanian Suppletionmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…), of uadere originally 'go' , make 'an impressive, terrifying, threatening, rapid, dangerous or showy advance (especially into dangerous situations)' , 2 in the singular and third person forms of the present masculine and feminine forms of mic 'small' . I reproduce the data in Table 1, transposed into IPA 5 (the place names are as given in the source): For comparison I give in Table 2 the -regular -paradigm of the word for 'dead' (see Atanasov 2009, maps 582 and 583): The example in Table 2 is fairly typical of Megleno-Romanian adjectives, most of which mark masculine singular by final -u (or zero, according to dialect and to phonological environment), feminine singular by -ǝ, and feminine plural by -i (< *-e); the original masculine plural ending, now generally deleted at the end of a phonological word, was -i, which originally produced various palatalizing and/or affricating effects on preceding root-final consonants (e.g., mworts < *ˈmortsi < *ˈmorti). There is a second, less numerous, inflexion class which only distinguishes number (and not gender) and is characterized by -i (< *-e) in the singular and usually zero in the plural (although originally the plural ending was *-i): e.g., sg ˈratsi 5.…”
Section: Two Suppletive Adjectives In Megleno-romanianmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Schemata are cognitive associations that represent the commonalities of the affected items, and their effects are similar to those of 'templates' , which are described by Bybee (1985) and Aski (1995). Schemata represent how language users may make sense of/categorize incoming data, and can be composed of any number of items, large or small.…”
Section: Grammatical/morphological Conditioning and Cognitive Schematamentioning
confidence: 99%