2004
DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.05.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Verbal short-term memory reflects the sublexical organization of the phonological language network: Evidence from an incidental phonotactic learning paradigm

Abstract: The nonword phonotactic frequency effect in verbal short-term memory (STM) is characterized by superior recall for nonwords containing familiar as opposed to less familiar phoneme associations. This effect is supposed to reflect the intervention of phonological long-term memory (LTM) in STM. However the lexical or sublexical nature of this LTM support is still debated. We explored this question by using an incidental phonological learning paradigm. We exposed adults and 8-year-olds to an artificial phonotactic… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
104
0
2

Year Published

2007
2007
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 97 publications
(110 citation statements)
references
References 40 publications
4
104
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Since the linguistic characteristics of the nonword sets were controlled (e.g., in terms of phonotactic frequency, number of phonological neighbours, plus the frequency with which particular phonemes occurred within the stimuli) and the allocation of these sets to each experimental condition was counterbalanced between participants, we can be confident that the highly reliable difference in ISR between familiarised and unfamiliar nonwords reflected the opportunity to learn their phonological forms. This indicates that verbal STM receives substantial support from long-term information about phonological sequences that occur within the language, even when this information is newly acquired on the basis of on a few presentations (for similar conclusions from single item repetition see Majerus, Linden, Mulder, Meulemans, & Peters, 2004). …”
Section: A C C E P T E D Accepted Manuscriptmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…Since the linguistic characteristics of the nonword sets were controlled (e.g., in terms of phonotactic frequency, number of phonological neighbours, plus the frequency with which particular phonemes occurred within the stimuli) and the allocation of these sets to each experimental condition was counterbalanced between participants, we can be confident that the highly reliable difference in ISR between familiarised and unfamiliar nonwords reflected the opportunity to learn their phonological forms. This indicates that verbal STM receives substantial support from long-term information about phonological sequences that occur within the language, even when this information is newly acquired on the basis of on a few presentations (for similar conclusions from single item repetition see Majerus, Linden, Mulder, Meulemans, & Peters, 2004). …”
Section: A C C E P T E D Accepted Manuscriptmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…As we have mentioned before, a number of studies suggest that the more detailed and finely grained the structure of the phonological network, the higher the efficiency of nonword segmentation and temporary representation (e.g., Metsala, 1999). This reliance on sublexical phonological knowledge in the item STM task was also directly controlled by contrasting nonwords with high or low phonotactic frequency patterns relative to English phonology; these nonwords differ according to sublexical frequency (frequency of phoneme co-occurrences) but also lexical frequency measures, nonwords containing more frequent phoneme associations having typically a higher number of lexical neighbours (e.g., Majerus et al, 2004;Thorn & Frankish, 2005).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A number of studies show that long-term phonological knowledge influences STM performance, as reflected by better recall for words of high versus low lexical frequency or for nonwords containing high versus low probability phonotactic patterns (e.g., Gathercole, Frankish, Pickering & Peaker, 1999;Majerus, Van der Linden, Mulder, Meulemans, & Peters, 2004;Thorn & Gathercole, 1999;Thorn & Frankish, 2005; see also Goh &Pisoni, 2003, andRoodenrys &Hinton, 2002, for related findings). Furthermore, although phonological STM capacity at Age 4 predicts vocabulary knowledge at Age 5, this relationship later reverses, with vocabulary knowledge at Age 5 predicting phonological STM capacity at Age 6 (Gathercole et al, 1992).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…familiar words differing from the nonword by a single phoneme (Majerus et al, 2004;Vitevitch & Luce, 1998). Serial order processing requirements were reduced given that only one item had to be maintained for each trial and given that, at the sublexical level, all items had the same short monosyllabic structure.…”
Section: Studymentioning
confidence: 99%