2011
DOI: 10.1002/aur.222
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Verbal problem‐solving in autism spectrum disorders: A problem of plan construction?

Abstract: Children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) adopt less efficient strategies than typically developing controls (TD) on verbal problem-solving tests such as the Twenty Questions Task. This study examined the hypotheses that this can be explained by differences in (i) planning processes or (ii) selective attention. Twenty-two children with ASD and 21 TD controls matched for age (M(age) = 13:7) and cognitive ability (M(FSIQ) = 96.42) were tested on an adapted version of Twenty Questions and two planning tasks. … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
9
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 329 publications
(664 reference statements)
2
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, it is important to recognise that the use of differing cognitive strategies in this group is a mark of variation, not deficiency: the adult participants with ASD in Williams et al’s (2012) study could complete tower tasks apparently without recourse to verbal strategies, so intervention in this case would be inappropriate. Where training with verbal protocols may be more warranted is in situations that demand specific use of verbal strategies: for instance, use of written cues improves problem-solving efficiency on the Twenty Questions task in children with ASD ( Alderson-Day, 2011 ). In the cases of SLI and ADHD, instructional training in private speech at earlier ages may serve to counteract apparent delays in verbal strategy use.…”
Section: Inner Speech In Atypical Populationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, it is important to recognise that the use of differing cognitive strategies in this group is a mark of variation, not deficiency: the adult participants with ASD in Williams et al’s (2012) study could complete tower tasks apparently without recourse to verbal strategies, so intervention in this case would be inappropriate. Where training with verbal protocols may be more warranted is in situations that demand specific use of verbal strategies: for instance, use of written cues improves problem-solving efficiency on the Twenty Questions task in children with ASD ( Alderson-Day, 2011 ). In the cases of SLI and ADHD, instructional training in private speech at earlier ages may serve to counteract apparent delays in verbal strategy use.…”
Section: Inner Speech In Atypical Populationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, efficient problem solving relies on executive functions (EFs); that is, the set of skills required to retain and manipulate information “on-line” during goal-directed tasks, such as planning, flexibility, selective attention, inhibition and working memory [Hill, 2004]. Two studies by Alderson-Day and colleagues studied the effects of these factors on TQT performance [Alderson-Day, 2011; Alderson-Day & McGonigle-Chalmers, 2011]. The typical TQT includes an array of pictures that do not change throughout the task, meaning that participants have to remember their questions “on-line” as they play [Mosher & Hornsby, 1966].…”
Section: Problem Solving In People With Asdmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When items cannot be removed, participants not only have to remember questions, but they also have to selectively attend to relevant information in the visual array. To parse out these demands, a second study by Alderson-Day [2011] provided participants with a written reminder of their questions when knocking down items was prohibited. This eliminated the need for additional questions in the ASD group—even though the visual demands of the task had not changed—implying a problem with memory for questions rather than attention.…”
Section: Problem Solving In People With Asdmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, tasks were not included if they did not assess the cognitive ability of thinking ahead, such as motor planning tasks, or tasks that were not commonly known in the planning literature and of which we, therefore, did not know whether they validly assessed planning. For example, one of the tasks that we did not include was the Question Discrimination and Plan Construction task used in Alderson-Day (2011), as this task was not used in any other ASD planning study and is not widespread in the planning literature.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%