1980
DOI: 10.3758/bf03334498
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Verbal discrimination learning and retention as a function of task and performance or observation

Abstract: Learning and retention of verbal materials were compared in high school students under perform (guessing with feedback) or observe (watching the performer) conditions. The correct response was randomly determined, or the shortest word, or the word that made a short message when linked to contiguous correct words. In acquisition, subjects did better when performing, females did better than males, and the shortest word group did better than the other groups. There was a variety of interactions in the retention t… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

1
4
0

Year Published

1980
1980
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

3
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 5 publications
(5 reference statements)
1
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The results of this experiment provide much better support than the previous similar but less systematically designed study (Marx et al, 1980) for the hypothesis that the reward process presumably more directly involved in acquisition of performed responses acts to strengthen such responses more than the cognitive processes involved in acquisition of observed responses . The supporting evidence comes not only from the reliably greater mean number of performed responses acquired, but also, and more strongly, from the interaction of perform/observe and task variables.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 56%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The results of this experiment provide much better support than the previous similar but less systematically designed study (Marx et al, 1980) for the hypothesis that the reward process presumably more directly involved in acquisition of performed responses acts to strengthen such responses more than the cognitive processes involved in acquisition of observed responses . The supporting evidence comes not only from the reliably greater mean number of performed responses acquired, but also, and more strongly, from the interaction of perform/observe and task variables.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 56%
“…This experiment continues the analysis of verbal discrimination learning as a function of task initiated in a recent experiment (Marx, Homer, & Marx, 1980).…”
mentioning
confidence: 70%
“…Students in each class were tested in pairs. One student in each pair performed(that is, actually made the responses) while the other student observed, in a manner previously used (e.g., Marx, Homer, & Marx , 1980). Performers paced themselves and wereallowed 5 min for the exercise.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research in the behaviourist tradition recommends that exposure to incorrect examples should be withheld on the grounds that learners may internalise and learn the wrong information (Skinner, 1961). Consistent with this idea, learners who observe others make wrong responses on a learning task—even if those errors are corrected—have the tendency to later mistake those errors as correct (Marx et al, 1980). Previous work also suggests that the incorrect representations that learners initially form about a given concept can proactively interfere with subsequent learning (Corral, Quilici, et al, 2019), even after the concept has been fully learned (Corral & Jones, submitted).…”
mentioning
confidence: 92%
“…This mixture condition has not typically been included in previous research on learning from examples (although see Booth et al, 2013; also see Taylor et al, 2016; Zhao & Acosta-Tello, 2016) but provides an important condition for evaluating theories. If subjects simply learn what they see (Marx et al, 1980; Skinner, 1961), then a greater number of correct examples should benefit learning, such that learning will be best for subjects who see only correct examples, followed by mixed examples, and worst for incorrect examples. However, if incorrect examples benefit learning by highlighting the properties of the to-be-learned concept that are not present in the example (Heemsoth & Heinze, 2014), then the condition involving incorrect examples should be most effective for learning, followed by mixed examples and correct examples.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%