2002
DOI: 10.2466/pms.2002.94.3.914
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Velocity, Occlusion, and Sex of Subjects in Coincidence of Anticipation

Abstract: The purpose of this study was to assess effects of velocity, occlusion distance, and sex on coincidence of anticipation. The Bassin anticipation timer was used to test 25 men and 25 women when velocity was .69 m/sec. and 1.38 m/sec. or "slow" and "fast", and occlusion distances of zero, 1.06 m and 2.12 m were "zero," "small," and "large" on a 4.25-m length of track. There were six test conditions. Analysis showed that increased occlusion distances produced increased absolute, constant, and variable error score… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
14
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
0
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…increasing speed). Increased speed equating to increased difficulty has been supported in many motor control paradigms including: line tracing [20], reciprocal motions [3], and incident-anticipation [21, 22]. The effects of resistance on motor tasks has been less definitive, where increased pen weight (resistance) increased the error of a reciprocal task [3], though resistance did not influence the error of a complex repeated motion [6].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…increasing speed). Increased speed equating to increased difficulty has been supported in many motor control paradigms including: line tracing [20], reciprocal motions [3], and incident-anticipation [21, 22]. The effects of resistance on motor tasks has been less definitive, where increased pen weight (resistance) increased the error of a reciprocal task [3], though resistance did not influence the error of a complex repeated motion [6].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The timing control in the occlusion condition requires sufficient cognitive load to estimate the TTC (DeLucia & Liddell, 1998;Jagacinski, Johnson, & Miller, 1983;Tresilian, 1995). The general consensus across numerous studies is that a shorter visible length will yield substantial timing errors, as participants must respond without a continuous view of the moving target, while a longer visible length will yield more accurate responses, as individuals can acquire more detailed information about the target (e.g., Harrold & Kozar, 2002;Payne, 1986;Whiting, Gill, and Stephenson, 1970). Therefore, larger RM (i.e., longer visible length) after the midpoint of the trajectory (i.e., after the target vanished) would aid timing judgment of the end of the trajectory.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While males have been found to be more accurate and less variable than women (Les et al, 2002;, there is also empirical evidence showing no gender effects (Harrold & Kozar, 2002;Millslagle, 2004;.…”
Section: Manual Asymmetry In a Complex Coincidence-anticipation Task:mentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Variables such as age (Benguigui, Broderick, & Ripoll, 2004;Lobjois, Benguigui, & Bertsch, 2005, stimulus speed (Coker, 2003;Harrold & Kozar, 2002;Teixeira, Lima, & Franzoni, 2005), gender (Les, Katene, & Fleming, 2002;Millslagle, 2004;, motorresponse complexity (Teixeira, 2006;Williams, Jasiewicz, & Simmons, 2001), and manual asymmetry (Cockerill, van Zyl, & Nevill, 1988;Coker, 2004) have been studied currently in the coincidenceanticipation literature.…”
Section: Manual Asymmetry In a Complex Coincidence-anticipation Task:mentioning
confidence: 98%