1989
DOI: 10.1017/s0954394500000193
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Variation in subject-verb concord in Early Modern English

Abstract: Determining the function of verbal -s in the Black English Vernacular (BEV) has been a major problem in sociolinguistics. Linguists have offered four answers to questions about the feature's origins and function, with -s seen as a case of hypercorrection, as a marker of durative/habitual aspect, as a variable marker of present tense (with the variation stemming from dialect mixture), and as a marker of historical present regardless of person and number. This article argues that confusion about -s results large… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
31
0

Year Published

1994
1994
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 107 publications
(33 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
2
31
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Compiled from data provided in 13. See Bailey, Maynor, and Cukor-Avila (1989) for an analysis of the effects of this constraint on the use of verbal -s in early modern English. 14.…”
Section: Summar Y and Conclusionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Compiled from data provided in 13. See Bailey, Maynor, and Cukor-Avila (1989) for an analysis of the effects of this constraint on the use of verbal -s in early modern English. 14.…”
Section: Summar Y and Conclusionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The NP > pronoun effect is also significant in that it aligns this variety with a diachronically recurrent constraint pattern for subject-verb concord in English (Bailey, Maynor, and Cukor-Avila 1989;Montgomery 1989Montgomery , 1994Tagliamonte 1997; Smith and Tagliamonte forthcoming). 3 The VARBRUL analysis for regularization to weren't is given in Table 5.…”
Section: On the Variable Incidence Of Was And Were Levelingmentioning
confidence: 57%
“…The syntactic and semantic reanalysis of zero and full/contracted copula forms by post-World War II Springville speakers bears a striking resemblance to another grammatical development within the present tense of be, the grammaticalization of be 2 (Bailey andMaynor 1987, 1989;Cukor-Avila and Bailey 1995), the near-categorical loss of present-tense verbal -s (Bailey, Maynor, and Cukor-Avila 1989;Cukor-Avila 1997), and the post-World War II reanalysis of had+past as a simple past-tense form (Cukor-Avila 1995;Cukor-Avila and Bailey 1995). 6 Bailey andMaynor (1987, 1989) argue that the emergence of invariant be+V+ing as a marker of habitual aspect was largely a consequence of three things: (1) structural pressures within the progressive system itself, which in English can have a number of meanings (habitual, durative, and future) that are not progressive at all; (2) the presence of variant auxiliary forms of be in English (e.g., is/º/be) that were not distinct from one another in function; and (3) the presence of an earlier invariant be in black folk speech, one that derived from the deletion of a preceding would and carried past habitual meaning.…”
Section: Cukor-avila / Stativity and Copula Absence In Aave 347mentioning
confidence: 95%
“…They point out that in earlier AAVE, -s and º both occurred in the plural as well as the singular, with their occurrence conditioned not only by the person/number of the subject but also by whether the subject was an NP (which favored -s) or a personal pronoun (which favored º); in other words, two functions were competing for the same form. In most Southern white vernaculars, this competition was resolved in favor of person/number agreement; Bailey, Maynor, and Cukor-Avila (1989) argue that in AAVE, it was resolved by the near elimination of the form for which the functions were competing. The work of Cukor-Avila (1995 provides strong support for these hypotheses and details the path by which -s disappeared.…”
Section: Cukor-avila / Stativity and Copula Absence In Aave 347mentioning
confidence: 99%