2015
DOI: 10.1002/ece3.1567
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Variation in signal–preference genetic correlations in Enchenopa treehoppers (Hemiptera: Membracidae)

Abstract: Fisherian selection is a within-population process that promotes signal–preference coevolution and speciation due to signal–preference genetic correlations. The importance of the contribution of Fisherian selection to speciation depends in part on the answer to two outstanding questions: What explains differences in the strength of signal–preference genetic correlations? And, how does the magnitude of within-species signal–preference covariation compare to species differences in signals and preferences? To add… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 91 publications
(239 reference statements)
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Stimuli were presented to a female by imparting them to a plant stem at an amplitude of 0.15 mm s −1 using a piezoelectric controller and actuator (Thorlabs, Newton, NJ, USA). Each bout varied from others only in dominant signal frequency, with all other signal values set to the mean of the population (Fowler‐Finn et al ., ). The playback stimuli had signal frequencies of 165 Hz, and steps of ± 2, 4, 6, 8, 10, 15, 20, 30, 40 Hz above and below this value.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 97%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Stimuli were presented to a female by imparting them to a plant stem at an amplitude of 0.15 mm s −1 using a piezoelectric controller and actuator (Thorlabs, Newton, NJ, USA). Each bout varied from others only in dominant signal frequency, with all other signal values set to the mean of the population (Fowler‐Finn et al ., ). The playback stimuli had signal frequencies of 165 Hz, and steps of ± 2, 4, 6, 8, 10, 15, 20, 30, 40 Hz above and below this value.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…). Selectivity is the PC summarizing variation in the aspects of the shape of the mate preference that are independent from the peak (Fowler‐Finn & Rodríguez, ; Fowler‐Finn et al ., ). They include responsiveness (the mean number of responses across all stimuli, which corresponds to the overall elevation of the preference functions), tolerance (how quickly the female response drops as the signal frequency deviates from the preferred signal values) and strength (the level of variation in responses between more and less preferred signal frequencies).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Pair formation involves male-female signal exchanges (duets), with strong female preferences for certain male signal features (Cocroft et al 2008). These signals and mate preferences likely represent a preference/trait mechanism because within-species genetic correlations between signals and preferred signal values are weak or absent (Fowler-Finn et al 2015), in spite of pronounced signal-preference coevolution between species (Rodríguez et al 2006;Cocroft et al 2008). Thus, a multiplicity of mechanisms plays a role in the process of divergence in this species complex, perhaps with different mechanisms making varying contributions at different stages.…”
Section: The Hard Cases: Behavioral Preferencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…In populations in which the mean male signal and female preference mismatch, temperature coupling would reinforce directional selection across temperatures—which would be expected in Columbia, MO, where the mean female peak preference averages ~30 Hz lower than the mean signal frequency. Sexual selection via female choice is important in shaping male signal variation in this clade (Rodriguez & Cocroft, ; Sullivan‐Beckers & Cocroft, ), and male E. binotata signals exhibit broad‐sense heritability (Fowler‐Finn et al., , ). Thus, if populations were to vary in the mean female preference, sexual selection across variable thermal environments could reinforce differential selection on male signals across populations, potentially contributing to rapid divergence across population (e.g.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Females become sexually receptive three to four weeks after the adult moult (Wood & Guttman, ), and so we assayed female preferences across the same range of temperatures one to two weeks after males(e.g. Fowler‐Finn, Kilmer, Cruz, & Rodríguez, ; Fowler‐Finn, Kilmer, Hallett, & Rodriguez, ; Fowler‐Finn & Rodriguez, ; Fowler‐Finn & Rodríguez, ; Kilmer et al., ; Rodríguez, Haen, Cocroft, & Fowler‐Finn, ).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%