2002
DOI: 10.1111/1467-9280.00476
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Variability and Detection of Invariant Structure

Abstract: Two experiments investigated learning of nonadjacent dependencies by adults and 18-month-olds. Each learner was exposed to three-element strings (e.g., pel-kicey-jic) produced by one of two artificial languages. Both languages contained the same adjacent dependencies, so learners could distinguish the languages only by acquiring dependencies between the first and third elements (the nonadjacent dependencies). The size of the pool from which the middle elements were drawn was systematically varied to investigat… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

30
539
3
7

Year Published

2002
2002
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
4
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 653 publications
(580 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
(13 reference statements)
30
539
3
7
Order By: Relevance
“…Other verb bias alternations such as the noun phrase/sentence complement alternation (e.g., the man wrote [that] the story [was interesting]; Garnsey, Pearlmutter, Myers & Lotocky, 1997) also involve variability in the post-verbal position (e.g., optional that), suggesting that such variation may play a role in verb class acquisition more generally. Humans also find it difficult to learn non-adjacent dependencies in artificial grammar learning tasks and variation can modulate this ability (Gómez, 2002;Gómez & Maye, 2005;Newport & Aslin, 2004). This is particularly relevant given that SRN models provide some of the best accounts of learning in these tasks (Chang, Janciauskas & Fitz, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Other verb bias alternations such as the noun phrase/sentence complement alternation (e.g., the man wrote [that] the story [was interesting]; Garnsey, Pearlmutter, Myers & Lotocky, 1997) also involve variability in the post-verbal position (e.g., optional that), suggesting that such variation may play a role in verb class acquisition more generally. Humans also find it difficult to learn non-adjacent dependencies in artificial grammar learning tasks and variation can modulate this ability (Gómez, 2002;Gómez & Maye, 2005;Newport & Aslin, 2004). This is particularly relevant given that SRN models provide some of the best accounts of learning in these tasks (Chang, Janciauskas & Fitz, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Learners are sensitive not just to conditional relations among adjacent elements, but also to relations among non-adjacent elements (e.g. [85,86]). That is, when presented with sequences like AXB, AYB and AZB, learners detect the relationship between A and B even though these items are not directly adjacent.…”
Section: How a Memory-based Approach Differs From A Probability-basedmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We believe, however, that it is unlikely that participants remembered a significant fraction of the exposure words. Words were never repeated more than once in exposure; the high variability of the vowel patterns (and therefore the particular words) is likely to have encouraged learning of the consonant patterns rather than learning of particular words (Gómez 2002). Indeed, although it is probably more difficult to remember 64 different words (in the Eight Sets group) than eight different words (in the One Set group), participants in the Eight Sets group showed better learning outcomes than those in the One Set group.…”
Section: Mechanisms Of Generalizationmentioning
confidence: 99%