1989
DOI: 10.1080/01638538909544719
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Using conversation MOPs in natural language interfaces

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

1989
1989
2008
2008

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 14 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Despite reports of behavioral differences for persons with varying expectations of future interaction, information-seeking goals and information-revealing desires (Berger & Kellermann, 1983, the findings reported here suggest that these differences are ones of routine flexibility; the routines are being flexibly deployed to adapt to differing circumstances but the conversations are still routine, being structured quite similarly in their progression. This normative sequencing is most likely why MOP-based, advice-giving systems have been successfully developed on computer systems: The computer and the user share the same representation for the discourse structure (Cullingford & Kolodner, 1986;Turner & Cullingford, 1989a, 1989b, much as two persons must share similar structures for talk to progress easily. It seems to be the case that persons flexibly deploy routines within relatively invariant (and routine) sequential progression when conversing.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Despite reports of behavioral differences for persons with varying expectations of future interaction, information-seeking goals and information-revealing desires (Berger & Kellermann, 1983, the findings reported here suggest that these differences are ones of routine flexibility; the routines are being flexibly deployed to adapt to differing circumstances but the conversations are still routine, being structured quite similarly in their progression. This normative sequencing is most likely why MOP-based, advice-giving systems have been successfully developed on computer systems: The computer and the user share the same representation for the discourse structure (Cullingford & Kolodner, 1986;Turner & Cullingford, 1989a, 1989b, much as two persons must share similar structures for talk to progress easily. It seems to be the case that persons flexibly deploy routines within relatively invariant (and routine) sequential progression when conversing.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Other work on using context in real-world applications includes the work of Bre´zillon, Gentile, Saker and Secron (1997) on context's impact on problem-solving for subway incident handling; Guha and Lenat's (1990) work on common sense reasoning and Pinto, Stephens and Bonnell's (1995) work on geographic reasoning in Cyc; O ztu¨rk and Aamodt's (1997) work on medical diagnosis; E. Turner's (1989) work on conversational control; and Va´mos' (1995) work on modeling domain expert knowledge, which resulted in patterns similar to very simple contextual schemas.…”
Section: Related Workmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Why should an inference based on a case, for example, be any different than one made based on a general plan derived from experience? It shouldn't (Martin, 1988;Shinn, 1988;Turner, 1988;Turner & Cullingford, 1989). One must still focus on a part of the plan.…”
Section: Control and Facilitation Of Case-based Inferencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The MEDLATOR has processes that are consistent with such an architecture. And current research is addressing way to make these guidelines more concrete for real-world problems (e.g., Martin, 1988;Shhm, 1988;Turner, 1988;Turner & Cullingford, 1989).…”
Section: Control and Facilitation Of Case-based Inferencementioning
confidence: 99%