2013
DOI: 10.1007/s10339-012-0534-0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Using a touch screen paradigm to assess the development of mental rotation between 3½ and 5½ years of age

Abstract: Mental rotation is an important spatial skill. However, there is controversy concerning its early development and susceptibility to intervention. In the present study, we assessed individual differences in the mental rotation abilities of children between 3 and 5 years of age, using a touch screen paradigm to simplify task demands. A figure or its mirror image was presented in 8 different orientations, and children indicated in which of two holes the figure would fit by touching one of the holes on the screen.… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

10
55
3

Year Published

2013
2013
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

4
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 89 publications
(68 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
10
55
3
Order By: Relevance
“…These results are in accordance with research on mental rotation, which revealed linear effects of rotation angle on 4-to 5-year-olds' response times (Marmor, 1975(Marmor, , 1977. However, whereas some mental rotation studies showed that less than half of the 4-year-olds can successfully rotate objects and performance increases considerably in preschool years (Estes, 1998;Frick, Ferrara, & Newcombe, 2013;Frick, Hansen, & Newcombe, 2013), no differences between 4-and 5-year-olds' response times were observed in the present scaling task. Thus, it is possible that mental rotation tasks pose higher cognitive demands, as they typically require children to differentiate mirror images, which may be especially challenging for young participants.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…These results are in accordance with research on mental rotation, which revealed linear effects of rotation angle on 4-to 5-year-olds' response times (Marmor, 1975(Marmor, , 1977. However, whereas some mental rotation studies showed that less than half of the 4-year-olds can successfully rotate objects and performance increases considerably in preschool years (Estes, 1998;Frick, Ferrara, & Newcombe, 2013;Frick, Hansen, & Newcombe, 2013), no differences between 4-and 5-year-olds' response times were observed in the present scaling task. Thus, it is possible that mental rotation tasks pose higher cognitive demands, as they typically require children to differentiate mirror images, which may be especially challenging for young participants.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…Vygotsky, 1978) to acquiring mental rotation skills and were able to process feedback information, whereas others (and especially 4-year-olds) were not cognitively ready to profit from feedback yet. In line with these results, a recent study (Frick, Ferrara, & Newcombe, 2013) showed that observational and manual experience improved mental rotation performance of 5-year-olds who were performing above chance, but did not improve the performance of 4-year-olds. Individual differences in cognitive readiness at age 5 also could explain the incongruent results regarding the trainability of mental rotation in previous studies (e.g., Marmor, 1977;Platt & Cohen, 1981).…”
Section: Running Head: Mental Rotation In 3-to 5-year-olds 20mentioning
confidence: 58%
“…As they get older, children may become better at translating observed movements into covertly activated (but not executed) action plans (Wilson, 2002). Indeed, recent research showed that by 5 years of age, both manual and observational experience increased accuracy in a mental rotation task (Frick, Ferrara, & Newcombe, 2013). In the present experiment, we investigated at which age infants would succeed at a mental rotation task after observational experience only.…”
Section: Mental Object Rotation!and Motor Development In 8-and 10-monmentioning
confidence: 90%