1990
DOI: 10.1177/105381519001400201
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Use of a Constant Time Delay Procedure in Teaching Preschoolers in a Group Format

Abstract: The primary purpose of this Investigation was to evaluate the effects of constant time delay (a near-errorless teaching strategy) in teaching preschoolers sight word reading in a group. A secondary purpose was to assess the effects of two attentional responses, specific (repeating the letter names) and general (looking at the stimulus cards) on observational and related, nontarget learning. A multiple probe design across pairs of words was used to evaluate the procedure. The results of the investigation indica… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
17
0
1

Year Published

1996
1996
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 44 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 10 publications
4
17
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…These results are similar to those of other studies that have been conducted using CTD procedures (e.g., Alig-Cybriwsky et al, 1990). One of the advantages of CTD noted is that FIGURE 2.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These results are similar to those of other studies that have been conducted using CTD procedures (e.g., Alig-Cybriwsky et al, 1990). One of the advantages of CTD noted is that FIGURE 2.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…Much of the research on CTD and other instructional procedures that has been conducted with preschoolage children has examined these procedures' effectiveness in a didactic format (Alig-Cybriwsky et al, 1990;Chiara, Schuster, Bell, & Wolery, 1995). While effective in terms of acquisition, these didactic approaches have not always been effective in terms of promoting generalization.…”
mentioning
confidence: 98%
“…First, all participants learned all academic behaviors directly taught with progressive time delay. This replicates previous research on the use of response prompting procedures in small group arrangements with preschool children with disabilities (e.g., Alig-Cybriwsky et al, 1990;Chiara, Schuster, Bell, & Wolery, 1995;Holcombe, Wolery, & Snyder, 1994;Holcombe, Wolery, Werts, & Hrenkevich, 1993;Wolery, Werts, Holcombe, Billings, & Vassilaros, 1993;Wolery, Holcombe, Werts, & Cipolloni, 1993). It adds to a smaller body of research that suggests acquisition of discrete academic skills also occur when children with disabilities are taught together in a small group with typically-developing peers (Ledford & Wolery, in press;Venn et al, 1996).…”
Section: Academic Behaviorssupporting
confidence: 67%
“…Using response prompting procedures during small group direct instruction has been effective for almost 200 participants with and without disabilities in at least 47 published studies (for a review, see Ledford, Lane, Elam, & Wolery, 2012), including 7 studies conducted with preschool-aged participants (e.g., Alig-Cybriwsky, Wolery, & Gast, 1990;Venn, Wolery, & Greco, 1996). If each participant is taught different behaviors during small group direct instruction, they may learn behaviors taught to their group mates, referred to as observational learning (Bandura, 1977).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Delayed prompting is referred to differently by researchers, and variations of the procedure have been labeled constant prompting, delayed cue, delayed matching, or as time delay procedures (Alig-Cybriwsky, Wolery, & Gast, 1990;Coleman-Martin & Heller, 2004;Gibson & Schuster, 1992;Johnson, 1977;Keel & Gast, 1992;Koscinski & Gast, 1993;Mattingly & Bott, 1990;Venn et al, 1993). The basic application of delayed prompting (regardless of the procedural name) is the same, although there are, of course, slight procedural variations.…”
Section: Research Review Of Errorless Learning Techniquesmentioning
confidence: 96%