1991
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01540.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Understanding Natural Cause: Children's Explanations of How Objects and Their Properties Originate

Abstract: An understanding of natural cause includes the realization that events can occur independently of human activity or intentions. It also often entails realizing that causal mechanisms can be nonobservable or nonobvious. The present research investigated to what extent children ages 4-7 have developed a concept of natural cause. Study 1 examined children's understandings of object origins (e.g., how the sun began); Study 2 probed children's causal understandings of object behaviors and properties (e.g., why rabb… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
35
1

Year Published

1994
1994
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

5
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 60 publications
(36 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
0
35
1
Order By: Relevance
“…On this view, children's generic language use reflects their expectation that animal concepts are more richly structured, coherent, and “kind-like” than artifact concepts. In support of this hypothesis, research has demonstrated that by the age of 3 or 4, children have naive theories about the ways in which animals and artifacts differ, including in their internal parts (Gelman, 1990; Simons & Keil, 1995), identity (Keil, 1989), inheritance (Hirschfeld, 1996; Springer, 1992), origins (Gelman & Kremer, 1991; Keil, 1989), self-generated movement (R. Gelman et al, 1995; Massey & R. Gelman, 1988), and spontaneous growth and healing (Backscheider et al, 1993; Rosengren et al, 1991).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…On this view, children's generic language use reflects their expectation that animal concepts are more richly structured, coherent, and “kind-like” than artifact concepts. In support of this hypothesis, research has demonstrated that by the age of 3 or 4, children have naive theories about the ways in which animals and artifacts differ, including in their internal parts (Gelman, 1990; Simons & Keil, 1995), identity (Keil, 1989), inheritance (Hirschfeld, 1996; Springer, 1992), origins (Gelman & Kremer, 1991; Keil, 1989), self-generated movement (R. Gelman et al, 1995; Massey & R. Gelman, 1988), and spontaneous growth and healing (Backscheider et al, 1993; Rosengren et al, 1991).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, direct comparisons of preschoolers' animal and artifact concepts have yielded differences on numerous dimensions, including: internal parts (R. Gelman, 1990; Simons & Keil, 1995), object identity (Keil, 1989), functionality (Greif, Kemler Nelson, Keil, & Gutierrez, 2006; Keil, 1994), inheritance (Hirschfeld, 1995; Springer, 1992), origins (Gelman & Kremer, 1991; Keil, 1989), self-generated movement (R. Gelman, Durgin, & Kaufman, 1995; Massey & R. Gelman, 1988), and spontaneous growth and healing (Backscheider, Shatz, & Gelman, 1993; Rosengren, Gelman, Kalish, & McCormick, 1991).…”
Section: Domain-specificity Of Generic Languagementioning
confidence: 99%
“…This perspective is supported by research showing that infants reliably distinguish between animals and artifacts in the first year of life (Mandler & McDonough, 1993), and that by preschool, children demonstrate a great deal of domain-specific theoretical knowledge about animal and artifact categories. For example, preschoolers acknowledge the role of human action in the creation of artifacts; they view animals as created by nature and artifacts as created by people (Gelman & Kremer, 1991), and they believe there is an important role for the intent of a human creator in assigning artifact identity (Bloom, 1996; Diesendruck, Markson, & Bloom, 2003; Gelman & Bloom, 2000) and function (Defeyter & German, 2003; German & Johnson, 2002). …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Carey, 1995). Previous work has demonstrated that younger preschoolers have many systematic domain-specific beliefs about animals and artifacts, including about how category membership is determined (Disendruck, Markson, & Bloom, 2003; Gelman & Kremer, 1991), category stability (Gelman & Wellman, 1991; Keil, 1989; Siegel & Callanan, 2007), characteristic properties (Simons & Keil, 1995), and causal processes (Hatano & Inagaki, 1994; Massey & Gelman, 1988; Rosengren, Gelman, Kalish, & McCormick, 1991). Yet, whether preschoolers have systematic ontological beliefs about category boundaries (e.g., viewing some categories as more objective and discrete than others) has not been addressed in prior work.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%