2010
DOI: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00011.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Understanding Interest in the Second Year of Life

Abstract: Infants (n = 24, mean age 13 months and n = 24, mean age 19 months) were tested on an extension of the method introduced by Tomasello and Haberl (2003) to examine the understanding of another person's interest in a novel object. Four objects were presented serially. For two objects, infants played with an experimenter. The infant played with one object alone, and the experimenter played with one object alone. Finally, all four objects were presented together, and the experimenter excitedly asked for one withou… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
6
1

Year Published

2011
2011
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
2
2

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 8 publications
1
6
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This suggests, then, that children in Experiment 1 had to overcome their a priori preferences for the familiarized novel objects to choose the supernovel objects. This is consistent with previous research that suggests that by 18–20 months children are able to overcome their own egocentric preferences to respond to adult requests (MacPherson & Moore, 2010; Repacholi & Gopnik, 1997). In this light, children’s strong bias for the supernovel object is even more striking.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 93%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…This suggests, then, that children in Experiment 1 had to overcome their a priori preferences for the familiarized novel objects to choose the supernovel objects. This is consistent with previous research that suggests that by 18–20 months children are able to overcome their own egocentric preferences to respond to adult requests (MacPherson & Moore, 2010; Repacholi & Gopnik, 1997). In this light, children’s strong bias for the supernovel object is even more striking.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 93%
“…The extant word learning literature is filled with examples in which young children use pragmatic cues in various ambiguous situations. For example, children make pragmatic inferences after having played with a particular object or game (e.g., Akhtar, et al, 1996; Liebal, Behne, Carpenter, & Tomasello, 2009; MacPherson & Moore, 2010) and after observing another’s preference for a particular item (e.g., Repacholi & Gopnik, 1997; Saylor, Sabbagh, Foruna, & Troseth, 2009) as well as when faced with gestures and other similar cues (e.g., Baldwin, 1991; Moore, et al, 1999; Tomasello & Akhtar, 1995) Clearly, in several situations, pragmatic cues can provided valuable information to solve the mapping problem.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…MacPherson and Moore (2010) directly contrasted what an adult knew with what the infant herself was familiar with. In their study, two objects were mutually familiar for adult and infant, a third object was new for the infant and a fourth was new for the adult, but “old” for the infant.…”
Section: The Role Of the Experiential Backgroundmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…showing that people attend more to novel stimuli in learning (e.g., Snyder et al, 2008;MacPherson & Moore, 2010;Horst et al, 2011), we use the general idea of Nematzadeh et al (2012) in allocating more strength to alignments that are more novel (cf. Eqn.…”
Section: Modeling Attention To Novelty Building On Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%