2003
DOI: 10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00230-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Understanding how input matters: verb learning and the footprint of universal grammar

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

11
169
1
6

Year Published

2006
2006
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
4
1

Relationship

3
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 217 publications
(194 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
11
169
1
6
Order By: Relevance
“…It seems likely that different semantic relationships may emerge at different stages of development. For example, the relationships between syntactic transitivity and causal events, and between ditransitivity and events of transfer, appear to hold cross-linguistically (Fisher et al, 1991;Gleitman, 1990) and even in the invented gesture systems of linguistically deprived deaf children (Feldman, Goldin-Meadow, & Gleitman, 1978;Goldin-Meadow & Mylander, 1998), suggesting that learners may be predisposed toward some of these fundamental constraints (although it is not clear whether this is the result of innate linguistic knowledge, Lidz, Gleitman, & Gleitman, 2003, or of more general pragmatic principles, Goldberg, 2004). Thus we would expect these factors to play a role in (mutually) constraining verb-syntax from an early stage in learning.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It seems likely that different semantic relationships may emerge at different stages of development. For example, the relationships between syntactic transitivity and causal events, and between ditransitivity and events of transfer, appear to hold cross-linguistically (Fisher et al, 1991;Gleitman, 1990) and even in the invented gesture systems of linguistically deprived deaf children (Feldman, Goldin-Meadow, & Gleitman, 1978;Goldin-Meadow & Mylander, 1998), suggesting that learners may be predisposed toward some of these fundamental constraints (although it is not clear whether this is the result of innate linguistic knowledge, Lidz, Gleitman, & Gleitman, 2003, or of more general pragmatic principles, Goldberg, 2004). Thus we would expect these factors to play a role in (mutually) constraining verb-syntax from an early stage in learning.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There is by now considerable evidence that such syntactic signatures provide a source of information that systematically cross-classifies the set of verbs within and across languages along lines of broad semantic similarity (Fisher, Gleitman & Gleitman, 1991;and for crosslinguistic evidence, Geyer, 1994;Lederer, Gleitman & Gleitman, 1995;Li, 1994). Furthermore, children are richly sensitive to these regularities in syntax-to-semantics mappings (Brown, 1957;Fisher, Hall, Rakowitz & Gleitman, 1994;Gropen, Pinker, Hollander, Goldberg & Wilson, 1989;Landau & Gleitman, 1985;Lidz, Gleitman, & Gleitman, 2003;Mintz & Gleitman, 2002;Naigles, 1990;Naigles, Gleitman & Gleitman, 1992;Naigles & Kako, 1993;Pinker, 1991;Snedeker, Thorpe & Trueswell, 2001;Waxman, 1994).…”
Section: A Proposal For How Mental Verbs Are Acquiredmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This single mapping would cover not only causal transitives but also those encoding contact, perception, motion, and spatial relationships. In fact, Fisher and colleagues have suggested that infants might begin verb learning with precisely this kind of broad mapping and only later develop LINKING LANGUAGE AND EVENTS 10 more specific intuitions (Fisher, Gertner, Scott, & Yuan, 2010;Fisher, 1996;Lidz, Gleitman, & Gleitman, 2003;Yuan, Fisher, & Snedeker, 2012). Second, children could believe (in addition to or instead of a general two-participant preference) that the transitive frame has multiple meaning 'clusters', each of which encodes a different specific event structure.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%