2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.072
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Uncovering phonological and orthographic selectivity across the reading network using fMRI-RA

Abstract: Reading has been shown to rely on a dorsal brain circuit involving the temporoparietal cortex (TPC) for grapheme-to-phoneme conversion of novel words (Pugh et al., 2001), and a ventral stream involving left occipitotemporal cortex (OTC) (in particular in the so-called “visual word form area”, VWFA) for visual identification of familiar words. In addition, portions of the inferior frontal cortex (IFC) have been posited to be an output of the dorsal reading pathway involved in phonology. While this dorsal versus… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
56
0
4

Year Published

2016
2016
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 45 publications
(69 citation statements)
references
References 85 publications
9
56
0
4
Order By: Relevance
“…We have shown previously that the VWFA holds a finely tuned orthographic representation that is not modulated by semantics or phonology (Glezer et al, 2009; Glezer et al, 2016), supporting the DRC model of reading (Coltheart, Rastle, Perry, Langdon, & Ziegler, 2001; Coltheart & Rastle, 1994) and other models that posit whole word lexical representations in which orthographic forms are stored as distinct units. In contrast, our results provide less support for models of reading such as the Triangle model (Seidenberg & McClelland, 1989) that have argued against the existence of an orthographic lexicon.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 63%
“…We have shown previously that the VWFA holds a finely tuned orthographic representation that is not modulated by semantics or phonology (Glezer et al, 2009; Glezer et al, 2016), supporting the DRC model of reading (Coltheart, Rastle, Perry, Langdon, & Ziegler, 2001; Coltheart & Rastle, 1994) and other models that posit whole word lexical representations in which orthographic forms are stored as distinct units. In contrast, our results provide less support for models of reading such as the Triangle model (Seidenberg & McClelland, 1989) that have argued against the existence of an orthographic lexicon.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 63%
“…Their prior experience with print supports increased brain activation (Sela, Izzetoglu, Izzetoglu, & Onaral, 2014). Thus, readers draw on prior print experiences for orthographic processing, involving the visual representation of words or groups of letters (Dehaene & Cohen, 2011; Fischer‐Baum, Bruggemann, Gallego, Li, & Tamez, 2017) to support rapid identification of familiar words without phonological processing (Glezer et al, 2016), facilitating fluent reading and comprehension.…”
Section: A Confluence Of Complexitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We found no significant correlations between phonological scores and neuroanatomical findings. A potential reason for this might be that only relatively small brain areas, suggested to be restricted to the left temporal and angular gyri (Glezer et al, 2016) process phonology and that the present analyses controlling for various nuisance variables impede small effects to become significant. Moreover, while both groups had more than 20 subjects, our overall sample size remained moderate, which in turn decreases power to observe small effects in the data.…”
Section: Associations Between Anatomy and Reading-related Skillsmentioning
confidence: 91%